Arousal, Attention, and Other Fundamental Functions





Fundamental functions, or disturbances of the “sensorium,” are a prerequisite to instrumental functions. They include arousal, basic attention, “mental control,” and psychomotor speed. The multimodal aspect of orientation to time and place, although dependent on memory, is also affected by attentional deficits, and is therefore discussed here. Alterations in fundamental functions decrease efficiency of cortical functioning, and hence affect the validity of the assessment of instrumental functions such as memory, language, or executive functions.


Fundamental Functions and Orientation


AROUSAL


Arousal is a psychophysiological readiness to react or respond to stimuli. Most patients requiring mental status examination do not need to be examined for arousal; however, some patients fluctuate in level of alertness and awareness. Basic arousal is present if there is nonspecific, nonreflex responsiveness to any verbal or physical stimuli. Arousal results from activation of the cerebral cortex and is centered in the ascending reticular activating system (ARAS) in the upper brain stem (primarily midbrain) with projections to the intralaminar and centromedian thalamic nuclei, the autonomic and endocrine systems in the dorsal hypothalamus, and, ultimately, the neocortex. Neurologists often divide disorders of arousal into those that affect the brain stem, with associated brain stem neurological findings, and those that more diffusely affect the neocortex. Primary states of arousal include alertness or wakefulness, lethargy or somnolence, sleep and hypnosis, obtundation, stupor or semicoma, and coma ( Table 7.1 ). In addition, there are related coma-like states that include the vegetative state, the minimally conscious state, akinetic mutism, coma vigil, and locked-in syndrome.



TABLE 7.1

States of Arousal



















































































































Stimulation for Arousal Verbal Response Eye Response Motor Response Physiology
Alert Normal Coherent response Eyes open and fixate/track/contact Moves and orients body Normal wakefulness,
normal EEG
Sleep, lethargy, somnolence Voice or physical- mild Coherent response Eyes open and fixate/track/contact Moves and orients body Slow wave or sleep EEG
Obtundation Loud voice or physical- strong Incoherent
(± mumbled)
Eyes open and fixate/track/± contact Moves ± orients body Slow wave EEG
Stupor or semicoma Physical- pain or noxious None
(may groan)
None
(eyes may flicker)
Withdrawal only; aimless movement Slow wave EEG
Coma None None None Reflex ± decorticate or decerebrate Slow wave EEG
Coma-Like States
Vegetative or apallic state (unaware of self, environment) None
None Eyes open; random eye movements; do not fixate/track/contact ± None Sleep-wake cycle, diffuse cortical damage; preserved brain stem, hypothalamic function
Minimally conscious
sate
Inconsistent responses None ± yes/no or intelligible speech Eyes open ± fixate/track/contact
None ± purposeful movements/ may follow simple commands Sleep-wake cycle, diffuse cortical damage; partial preservation of conscious awareness
Apathetic akinetic mute (look lethargic or somnolent) Inconsistent responses
(± orient to sound)
None spontaneously; minimal responses ± short, soft/whispered
May have eyes open and fixate/track/± contact
(± vertical gaze palsy)
Minimal but definite movements Lesion in midbrain, subthalamus, or frontal-ACG interrupts ARAS
Coma vigil
(look awake day and night)
Inconsistent responses
(± septal rage)
None spontaneously;
minimal responses ± short, soft/whispered
Eyes open and fixate/track/± contact Minimal but definite movements Lesion in septal region, anterior hypothalamus, frontal-ACG or bilateral OFC
Decreased Motivation States
Apathy Normal Minimal responses ± short, soft/whispered Eyes open and fixate/track/contact Minimal but definite movements Variable, may be normal
Abulia (difficult to distinguish from apathy, but more severe and normal mood) Normal Minimal responses ± short, soft/whispered Eyes open and fixate/track/±contact Minimal but definite movements Variable, may be normal
Catatonia Normal Minimal responses ± short, soft/whispered Eyes open ± fixate/track/contact
Minimal but definite movements; postures, catalepsy Variable, may be normal
Conditions Mistaken for Coma
Locked-in syndrome Normal None Eyes open ± fixate/track/contact;
blink movements (may communicate by blinking)
Reflex ± decorticate or decerebrate Variable, may be normal
Brain death None None None None No cortical or brain stem functions/reflexes (e.g., gag or corneal) and positive apnea test

ACG , anterior cingulate gyrus; ARAS , ascending reticular activating system; EEG , electroencephalogram; OFC , orbitofrontal cortex.


The primary arousal states vary in responsiveness to environmental stimuli. Patients with lethargy or somnolence tend to drift off if not stimulated, and those with obtundation require vigorous or greater than normal verbal or physical stimulation to arouse. Patients with stupor or semicoma are limited to responses to noxious or painful stimuli by groaning or withdrawal to pain, and those with coma are totally unresponsive except for reflex action, including decorticate or decerebrate posturing, depending on whether the dysfunction is above or below the red nucleus in the midbrain, respectively. The definition of coma additionally specifies the absence of normal sleep-wake cycles documented on electroencephalography. These categories of disturbed arousal are not discrete, and there is a continuum of arousal from alertness to deep coma. In contrast, coma-like states, such as vegetative state and minimally conscious state, have some apparent responsivity, with eye openings and sleep-wake cycles.


ATTENTION



“Everyone knows what attention is. It is the taking possession by the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought. Focalization, concentration, of consciousness are of its essence. It implies withdrawal from some things to deal effectively with others and is a condition, which has a real opposite in the confused, dazed, scatterbrained state, which in French is called distraction, and Zerstreutheit in German.” William James, The Principles of Psychology, Vol. 1, Chapter XI. Attention. 1890, pg 404.


Basic attention is selective attention. As explained by William James, attention is the ability to focus and concentrate arousal and mental activity on a targeted external or internal stimulus to the exclusion of others, that is, to avoid being distracted by extraneous stimuli. Like arousal, attention works by enhancing neocortical processing and is a prerequisite for effective instrumental cognitive abilities. Attention is more of a distributed system than arousal, and many areas of the brain can disrupt attention. Attention depends on the brain stem, both arousal from the ARAS and the mechanism for actual movement of attention; the reticular nucleus of the thalamus for modulating and “gating” the sensory input so that selectivity emerges; the parietal cortex for ipsilateral disengaging and contralateral shifting attention and its spatial distribution (particularly right parietal); and the prefrontal cortex for the mental control or complex aspects of shifting, maintaining, and dividing attention ( Fig. 7.1 ). Given the distributed system mediating attention, this cognitive process is quite vulnerable to metabolic disruptions. Patients with abnormal attention have difficulty concentrating and experience impersistence, ease of distraction, and increased vulnerability to interference. Abnormal attention is the hallmark of delirium (see Chapter 3), the most common cause of behavioral disturbance among hospitalized elderly patients.




Fig. 7.1


Attentional pathways. The diagram illustrates the processes of attention including engagement, disengagement, maintenance, interaction with alerting, and the ability to move the attentional “spotlight.”


MENTAL CONTROL


Mental control is the management of attention. It is a heterogeneous concept that includes complex attention and short-term or working memory. Complex attention involves the ability to maintain attention, divide attention between two or more stimuli, shift attention between stimuli, vigilance or readiness to shift attention, and spatial attention. These complex attentional activities interact with working memory, an aspect of executive functions (see Chapter 13). Maintaining or sustaining attention is altered in most clinic or bedside tests of attention. Divided attention, however, can be quite difficult to test independent of executive functions. The ability to shift attention involves mechanisms for engaging, disengaging, and actual movement of the attentional spotlight. Vigilance has an optimal level; for example, hypervigilance leads to easy distraction to irrelevant stimuli, as is the case in delirium tremens. Spatial disorders of attention, such as hemispatial neglect and Balint syndrome, are discussed in Chapter 10 .


PSYCHOMOTOR SPEED


Speed of processing and latency of response onset are cognitive abilities that can be very sensitive to aging, white matter disease, frontal lobe disorders, and other conditions. “Psychomotor” speed combines or conflates, two general neurological processes, basic cognitive speed and basic motor speed. The former is a mental decision-making process, and the latter is basic motor reaction time or speed of movement. Psychomotor speed is particularly a function of the basal ganglia (striatum, globus pallidus, subthalamic nucleus, substantia nigra) and their white matter connections from frontal lobe regions (dorsolateral, orbitofrontal, anterior cingulate, supplementary motor area). These frontal cortical areas promote action through input to the striatum (caudate nuclei and putamen), which, in turn, directly inhibits the globus pallidus (and, less directly, the subthalamic nucleus), thus releasing the thalamus and other structures for action selection and motor execution. Some neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders result in increased speed, but most slow basic mental decision times, both response choice and response selection, even beyond basic motor reaction time. Slowed psychomotor speed and delayed latency of response initiation are common in delirium, frontal conditions, advanced dementia, and, of course, parkinsonian disorders; whereas increase speed may occur in hyperkinetic delirium and agitated, anxious, or manic patients.


ORIENTATION


As discussed in Chapter 5 , orientation is a very sensitive, although nonspecific, measure of an altered mental status. Orientation to time and place is a multimodal characteristic and not a specific property of the brain. Orientation to time is usually the most sensitive, followed by orientation to place. Many people are frequently mildly disoriented in time and place, off a day or so on the date or confused about the building location or floor, which may vary depending on the context or time of day. Pathological disorientation for time and place occur from disturbances of attention such as delirium, from memory disorders such as dementia, and from other cognitive deficits in language, semantics, perception, or executive abilities. In contrast to disorientation to time and place, disorientation for person or personal identity is not usually a manifestation of cognitive impairment and likely reflects a primary psychiatric disorder, such as a dissociative state.


Testing of Fundamental Functions and Orientation


AROUSAL


A disturbance of arousal is usually evident on initial patient presentation. Is the patient alert or dull, wide awake or drowsy? If the patient is not alert, can he or she be aroused to full awareness or will arousal produce only partial awakening? If the patient is aroused, can attention be sustained or does the patient drift back into sleep? Can the patient keep his or her eyes open, or do they remain shut; are the eyes fixed or do they follow movements?


If a disturbance of arousal is suspected, the examiner evaluates four aspects of the arousal examination. First, the examination assesses whether the patient is awake and responding to the environment. Alternatively, he or she may need stimulation for arousal. For verbal stimulation, clinicians loudly call the patient’s name while tapping them. If physical stimulation is necessary, clinicians apply sternal pressure or pinch the Achilles tendon. A description of the extent of stimulation necessary for responsiveness is a gauge of the level of wakefulness and provides useful data for later comparison. Second, the examiner notes the type of responsiveness to stimulation. Evaluate whether the patient maintains eyes open and visually fixates, tracks stimuli, or attains eye contact. Note the presence of any verbal responses, reactive movements to stimulation, reflex actions, or posturing. The examiner can semiquantify the eye, verbal, and motor responses with various scales, such as the Glasgow Coma Scale ( Table 7.2 ). This assessment for arousal must take place in the context of a thorough neurological examination that particularly focuses on the presence of brain stem signs, such as pupillary size, symmetry, and reactivity; extraocular movements including oculocephalic reflexes (“Doll’s Eyes Maneuver”), respiratory pattern, and long tract findings or upper motor neuron signs.


May 9, 2021 | Posted by in NEUROLOGY | Comments Off on Arousal, Attention, and Other Fundamental Functions

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access