, Marcy Willard1 and Helena Huckabee1
(1)
Emerge: Professionals in Autism, Behavior and Personal Growth, Glendale, CO, USA
Abstract
While individuals with ASD are characterized by challenges communicating and reciprocally interacting with others, it is often weaknesses in attention, memory, or executive functioning that underlie difficulties with academic or vocational success. Specifically, persons with autism typically manifest with selective attention to particular stimuli of most interest and have difficulty shifting their attention required to multitask. Of profound importance is the fact that it’s essential to attend to something in order to encode the event or item and be able to remember it later. In this manner, attending is the first step required to potentially learn something new. Subsequently, weaknesses in memory include remembering complex information (Neuropsychology 20:21–29, 2006), spatial working memory, relational memory (Neuropsychology 27:615–627, 2013), episodic memory, and time-based prospective memory. These memory difficulties significantly impact what an individual learns. Executive functioning pertains to an important cluster of skills which, like attention, are mediated by the frontal lobe and include initiating, organizing, and sequencing information. Frequent challenges in these areas associated with autism include problems generating ideas, responding inflexibility or perseverating, as well as deficits in planning. Challenges in these areas can often be just as debilitating as social weaknesses for an individual with autism. Hence, the importance of assessing, remediating, or compensating for these challenges cannot be overstated in order to help promote success at home, in school, and the workplace.
Keywords
Attention in ASDExecutive functions in ASDMemory in ASDSplinter skills: an outdated termExceptional abilities: an updated termSustained attention in ASDShifting attention in ASDFocused attention in ASDJoint attention in ASDNeuropsychological assessment of autismCan you have ASD and ADHD?Assessing for Attention
Vignette #7 Madeline: Assessment of Focused, Sustained, and Shifting Attention in a Child with Social Skills Deficits
Madeline, age 7, was referred for an evaluation at Emerge to rule out the presence of AD/HD. Her school recommended testing as Madeline had difficulty paying attention, was impulsive, and had meltdowns as she became easily upset with peers in the classroom. Madeline often claimed “nobody likes me!” but when asked “why do you think that is?” she seemed confused and remarked “Because I have a red scratch on my leg!” Parents reported that Madeline had an excellent memory but sometimes appeared to be “checked out” and did not pay attention. She had particular difficulty when she was engaged in a game or movie, and it was time to transition to another activity. In testing, Madeline presented with a High Average cognitive profile across all domains. She was very active and impulsive but maintained her attention with breaks every half hour. On the Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA), a test of sustained attention in the absence of immediate reinforcement, Madeline scored Above Average across all domains, even while she complained, “I made 12 mistakes!” (Table 11.1).
Table 11.1
Assessing for attention
Associated area |
1. Attention |
•Focused: adults may have difficulty obtaining attention of people with ASD as they may prefer to attend to objects or ideas of their own interest and ignore the social or environmental stimuli |
•Selective and Over-Selective: individuals with ASD tend to predominantly focus on restricted interests—may have over-selective attention, perseverating on specific interests for prolonged periods |
•Sustained: sustained attention may or may not be okay in ASD Typically sustained attention is a deficit in ADHD |
•Shifting: challenges with multi-tasking, establishing, and maintaining mental set This is typically challenging with ASD |
• Joint Attention : a process of engaging and shifting attention for social interaction (typically impaired in ASD) |
•Consider results from: ADT-II, Conners, T.O.V.A., and Observations of behaviors on the ADOS-II and WISC-V; Executive Functioning Measures |
Full measure names |
Conner’s Rating Scale Third Edition (Conners 3) |
Conner’s Continuous Performance Test, Second Edition (CPT-II) |
Conner’s Kiddie Continuous Performance Test, (K-CPT) |
Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA) |
Wepman’s Auditory Discrimination Test, Second Edition (ADT-II) |
General attention observations during cognitive testing |
Observations during ADOS-2 regarding attention during conversation/play |
Results from Executive Functioning measures |
Madeline had difficulty on a task of shifting and sequencing, the Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT ), and worked more slowly than would be expected. On the Tower of London (TOL-2), Madeline broke rules on three of ten puzzles and presented with inflexibility in her attempts to solve problems. She also had some perseveration and difficulty shifting set on the Wisconsin Card Sort Test (WCST ) , but this score was within age expected ranges.
Madeline was given the ADOS-2 based on her reported social struggles . While she played and conversed; making eye contact, and enjoying social interaction, she was inflexible in her play. Madeline had trouble taking a character as an agent of action and spent more time setting up the toys. She was a bit bossy with the examiner, telling her who to be and what to do. Madeline demonstrated significant challenges in understanding social relationships and told many stories about her experiences that seemed unlikely to be true. When the examiner shared something about herself, Madeline often responded with “I know.” Madeline’s own stories included reporting owning a leopard as a pet. These responses indicate that she does not know how to interact reciprocally; spontaneously share, and relate to the experiences of others.
Taken together, Madeline’s profile was indicative of an Autism Spectrum Disorder and not AD/HD. Her challenges were not in sustained or focused attention at all, though shifting attention and flexibility were problems, consistent with ASD. Madeline began to receive ABA support at school through her district’s Board Certified Behavior Analyst (BCBA ) and a Functional Analysis aided in the development of a behavior support plan. She began to participate in social skills training in a group setting. Her behavior improved substantially across social and school settings.
Attention Assessment
The recognition of co-occurrence of AD/HD with ASD is aptly captured in DSM-5 which permits comorbid diagnoses of both conditions. Indeed, studies have found similar structural brain abnormalities in those with ASD and AD/HD when compared to controls (Matson, Rieske, & Williams, 2013). In contrast to this literature, diagnostic criteria in the DSM-IV reflected the historical view that a diagnosis of ASD preempts and therefore precludes an additional diagnosis of AD/HD. This historical assumption is no longer considered valid as it was revisited and changed in the DSM-5 which permits comorbid diagnosis (Matson et al., 2013). Research indicates that AD/HD and ASDs are co-occurring 14–78 % (Gargaro et al., 2011). This is a wide range but indicates that many individuals with ASD struggle with attention problems. For this reason, it is very important to thoroughly assess attention skills when evaluating for an ASD.
As will be comprehensively reviewed in the Chap. 13, differential diagnosis of AD/HD requires an assessment of disinhibition and social reciprocity. If the child is disinhibited and lacks social reciprocity, he or she likely has both AD/HD and ASD. Mayes et al. (2012) state that ASD and AD/HD are “neurobiological disorders with similar underlying neuropsychological deficits.” (p. 283) Children with comorbid symptoms have more difficulty inhibiting inappropriate responses when compared with children who have ASD alone (Mannion & Leader, 2013). Those with ASD alone demonstrate better inhibition, flexibility, working memory, and planning skills than those with comorbid diagnosis (Sinzig, Bruning, Morsch, & Lehmkuhl, 2008) although these skills are still often challenging. Deprey and Ozonoff & Corbett et al. (2009) found that hyper vigilant attention and internal distractibility are more common in ASD while lack of focus and distractibility by external stimuli is characteristic of AD/HD.
The authors of this text observe that many children with ASD exhibit intermittent challenges with attention and executive functioning skills while those with comorbid diagnoses have significant deficits in sustained attention, inhibition, and focus. Individuals with AD/HD who do not have ASD may have some social challenges influenced by impulsivity, inattention, and hyperactivity. However, when focused, individuals with only AD/HD should be able to engage in pretend play and creative activities like telling a story or acting out a cartoon. Children with ASD alone tend to have less pervasive problems with attention. They can attend when given appropriate reinforcement. Sustained attention can be assessed via continuous performance testing and with rating scales completed by parents, teachers, and individuals. These may include the TOVA, BASC-2, BRIEF, or Conners AD/HD scales.
Attention Assessment as It Applies to Autism
Attention can be broadly defined as information processing mechanisms that mediate perceptual selectivity (Keehn, Shih, Brenner, Townsend, & Müller, 2013). Problems with attention have historically been considered as an associated or secondary deficit when present with ASD. Research reveals, however, that impairments represent early and lifelong abnormalities in efficiently modulating attention networks for individuals with ASD (Keehn, Müller & Townsend, 2013).
Attention mechanisms represent the confluence of external stimuli processed through bottom-up, sensory afferent nerve signals received in the posterior brain regions as well as internal, goal-directed processes originating in the anterior/frontal brain regions and executed through efferent, top-down signals (Kandel, Schwartz, & Jessell, 1991). Sensory processing assessment and therapies (Ayres, 2005) focus on the former bottom-up processes while behavior analytic assessments (Iwata et al., 1990) and therapies focus on both processes through classical and operant conditioning paradigms (Barlow, 2002; Cooper, Heron, & Hewerd, 2013).
In light of the fact that millions of brain fibers are sensory processing assessment and therapies (Ayres, 2005) focus on the former bottom-up processes while behavior analytic assessments (Iwata et al., 1990) and therapies focus on both processes through classical and operant conditioning paradigms. In light of the fact that millions of brain fibers are connecting the posterior and anterior cortical regions, perceptual selectivity or attention rarely consists exclusively of top-down or bottom-up processes but rather successful and adaptive information processing requires the integration of both processes (Keehn et al., 2013).
As previously mentioned, AD/HD and ASDs co-occur in a wide range of 14–78 % of the population diagnosed with ASD (Gargaro et al., 2011). Such a range in findings likely reflects that, in addition to the wide ASD span of various symptoms and severities, attention problems are multifaceted and responsible for a distinct set of cognitive processes. These processes include alerting, orienting, and executive control networks (Petersen & Posner, 2012; Posner & Petersen, 1989). Furthermore, research on attention indicates that attention networks each have their own developmental trajectories resulting in the emergence and strengthening of various attention skills into adulthood. Often these trajectories are delayed or attention skills may be present but significantly less efficient for individuals with ASD. All these complexities emphasize the importance of individually assessing for the occurrence and nature of attention deficits when evaluating for an ASD.
The authors of this text observe that most individuals with ASD exhibit challenges with attention and executive functioning skills evidenced in selective attention to highly preferred interests or activities and/or problems with flexibility, ideational fluency, planning, or central coherence. Nonetheless, those with ASD and not AD/HD demonstrate better inhibition, flexibility, working memory, and planning skills than those with both ASD and AD/HD (Sinzig et al., 2008). Individuals with comorbid diagnoses of ASD and AD/HD have additional deficits in sustaining attention in the absence of immediate reinforcement. Individuals with AD/HD but not ASD may exhibit some social challenges as a result of being impulsive, inattentive, or hyperactive. Such social difficulties look different and may appear like obnoxiousness, impulsive aggressive acts toward peers, and a lack of body awareness that could interfere with social functioning. Some children with AD/HD may have some difficulty understanding emotions, challenges with rejection or academic punishment for over-active behavior, or even failed friendships from impulsive aggression. When focused, however, individuals with only AD/HD should be able to engage in reciprocal activities such as pretend play or conversation and they should exhibit synchronized verbal and gestural communication in creative activities such as telling a story or acting out a cartoon. The following discussion provides further details on attentional networks, skills, developmental trajectories, and comorbid deficits found with ASD. In addition, assessment measures appropriate for each attention process as well as recommendations for associated weaknesses or deficits are included.
Alerting Network
The alerting network is responsible for achieving and maintaining a state of sensitivity to incoming information (Keehn et al., 2013). Parents can attest to the fact that newborn infants respond only to internal information such as hunger or pain and often actually sleep best when there is a high level of external noise or activity. Such endogenous wakefulness is referred to as tonic alertness and describes general arousal. Tonic alertness in infants develops rapidly between 2 and 24 weeks. Subsequently, the autonomic nervous system continues to develop across the life span. Research on persons with ASD indicates the possibility of separate subgroups representing individuals with either hyper- or hypo-arousal . These two disparate subgroups are captured in the ADOS assessment where the examiner may observe and record the participant’s overall level of engagement, attention, excitability, and alertness ranging from over-responsive to lethargic. Clinically, individuals with ASD and hyperarousal are subject to being easily distracted by any form of internal or external stimuli and likely also meet criteria for AD/HD. Deprey and Ozonoff & Corbett et al. (2009) found that lack of focused attention skills and distractibility by external stimuli is characteristic of AD/HD. Furthermore, Mayes et al. (2012) found that children with comorbid symptoms of both ASD and AD/HD have more difficulty with inhibitory performance when compared with children who have ASD alone (Mannion & Leader, 2013) which may again reflect the hyperarousal subgroup. Alternately, some have hypothesized that chronic hyperarousal may actually result in increased overselective attention (Liss, Saulnier, Fein, & Kinsbourne, 2006); a tendency to perseveratively regard, review or rehearse high preference internal or external stimuli.
The second component of the alerting network is phasic alertness which refers to a transient response to a behavioral or experimental cue (Keehn et al., 2013). This is a reflexive physiological response to a change in the environment rather than a volitional reaction. Phasic alertness is modulated by the level of tonic alertness and tonic alertness or overall arousal influences task performance. Phasic orienting responses develop rapidly during the first year of life, efficiency, and speed of phasic alerting appear to develop into early school-age years, and may reach adult levels by age 7–8 years of age (Cycowicz, Friedman, & Rothstein, 1996). Electrophysiological measures have demonstrated atypical phasic alerting in individuals with ASD and findings suggest modulation of phasic alerting mechanisms may be dysfunctional in ASD (Keehn et al., 2013). This in turn is likely related to marked insensitivity to novel stimuli often seen in individuals with ASD.
Assessment of response to novel stimuli can be gleaned from the Wechsler Cancellation and Symbol Search tasks . Like most assessment tasks or protocols, these measures assess multiple constructs; the Symbol Search also being a measure of processing speed. Nonetheless, observation and review of the quality of performance, specifically, any errors, and the type of errors, can shed light on whether they are insensitive to novel stimuli. Parents or family members may also express concern that the individual with ASD ignores presents under the Christmas tree or fails to notice a new student in the classroom. A partner may complain their mate with ASD does not notice droopy plants that need watering or a special outfit they wear. These challenges may reflect problems with phasic orienting.
Conversation activities from the ADOS as well as clinical observations to the examiner’s verbal efforts to build rapport can shed light on the participant’s response to novel auditory stimuli. If a verbally fluent participant fails to comment appropriately or at all when the topic of conversation changes, even in the absence of emotional content, it may be indicative of a fundamental impairment in phasic orienting. The Cancellation and Symbol Search subtests combine with other measures to assist in assessing focused attention skills more generally. The Comprehensive Trail Making Test may yield valuable information on the participant’s distractibility to various visual stimuli.
The voluntary maintenance of alertness in the absence of immediate reinforcement is called sustained attention. Sustained attention increases rapidly from 2 to 6 months of age (Richards, 1997); and then increases significantly again from 3 to 6 years of age; continuing to develop into late childhood and adolescence, and reaching adult-like levels around 12 years of age (Lin, Hsiao, & Chen, 1999). Deficits in sustaining attention are often present within the population of individuals with ASD and, when present, warrant the added diagnosis of AD/HD either Inattentive or Combined Type. Sustained attention can be assessed directly via continuous performance testing (CPT) using the TOVA or Gordon. Subjective rating scales can be helpful; completed by parents, teachers, and individuals, including: BASC-2 or Conners’ AD/HD scales or the CBCL. Continuous performance testing measures are advantageous for providing computer captured reaction time, variations in reaction time as well as errors of omission and commission. While rating scales are clearly influenced by the perceptions of the rater, they do afford the possibility of obtaining information about the participant in other environments beyond the testing room.
Attention Recommendations
Strategies to boost or calm alertness in the face of hypo- or hyper- arousal , respectively, could include the following: sensory integration therapy, a rich sensory diet to improve bottom-up responsiveness to external stimuli, removal of irrelevant stimuli to reduce distractibility, having a consistent and predictable schedule to reduce uncertainty which may heighten arousal, and therapies to promote calming or soothing techniques. These calming techniques might include deep breathing, frequent breaks, and sensory activities often involving deep pressure. Further treatments might include cognitive and behavioral therapies to promote top-down goal-directed activities and responses. Pharmacological therapies may also be considered for problems with hypo- or hyper-arousal (Ozonoff & Corbett et al., 2009). Clearly, stimulant medications are likely to enhance arousal while benzodiazepines, neuroleptic medications, or antihypertensive drugs, for example, would be expected to reduce arousal levels. A psychiatrist can best advise individuals interested in considering these biophysiological strategies.
Strategies to boost weak or deficient phasic alerting are likely to focus on enhancing the relevant behavioral or environmental cues. Parents and teachers of children with ASD who have problems with phasic orienting should be aware that they are likely to require multiple visual and/or auditory prompts to respond to relevant or novel changes in the environment. Text books or reading material that employs bold print to highlighted vocabulary words or side bars to emphasize key concepts may be especially helpful to such individuals. Spouses, partners, or roommates may choose to adopt patient and deliberate strategies to orient their loved ones attention to important environmental cues to avoid being disappointed or incensed when special details are repeatedly overlooked.
Strategies to improve sustained attention skills include consideration of pharmacological therapies mentioned above as well as behavioral therapies and environmental modifications. Behavioral strategies can take on many dimensions from provision of tangible or social positive reinforcement designed to boost arousal and enhance learning as well as self-awareness and self-monitoring strategies to build skills in reviewing performance or scanning the environment. Environmental modifications include simplification of work or domestic settings to minimize clutter that could otherwise be distracting as well as implementing regular and frequent breaks to provide opportunities for movement or selective sensory stimulation to boost arousal and improve attention. Histogram data from the TOVA or other CPT task can suggest the maximum duration the individual is likely to be able to sustain attention for before they begin to miss information, respond impulsively, or attend sporadically.
Orienting Network
Shifting Attention
Posner, Walker, Friedrich, and Rafal (1984) defined visuospatial orienting as disengaging, shifting, and reengaging attention; or more parsimoniously as shifting attention. While phasic alerting mechanisms respond homogeneously across the visual field, orienting visual attention involves processing over a localized area (Mangun & Hillyand, 1988). Orienting is associated with the neurotransmitter acetylcholine (ACh) and improves proportionally to ACh levels (Thiel, Zilles & Fink, 2005). Orienting efficiency increases between the ages of 4 and 7 years (Mezzacappa, 2004), however, some research indicates no improvement from age 6 to adulthood (Rueda, Fan et al., 2004).
Individuals with ASD have consistently demonstrated deficits in orienting visual attention including findings that infants later diagnosed with ASD did not orient to visual stimuli as often (Baranek, 1999), did not orient to people or their voices as frequently (Maestro et al., 2002), and did not orient to their name as often as either typical controls or a developmental disability control group (Osterling & Dawson, 1994; Osterling et al., 2002). More research also found that children and adolescents with ASD were slower to shift attention (Keehn et al., 2010). Additional research found that individuals with ASD can use volitional control to orient attention, but this skill is also atypical compared with typical peers (Haist et al., 2005).
Research on disengagement of attention as the preliminary component involved in shifting attention shows individuals with ASD exhibit significantly increased latencies to disengage visual attention compared with matched children with Down syndrome or typical development (Landry & Bryson, 2004). In other words, children with ASD do not efficiently shift attention. Alarmingly, studies of infants at high risk for developing ASD, because of an older sibling with ASD, found that every single child that exhibited increased difficulties disengaging attention between 6 and 12 months received an ASD diagnosis at 24 months (Zwaigenbaum et al., 2005). Not all results are consistent yet it appears that disengagement of attention in ASD, when present, persists across the life span.
Problems shifting attention are readily assessed as part of a comprehensive evaluation. ADOS tasks including Response to Name, Initiating Joint Attention and Responding to Joint Attention clearly involve disengaging and shifting attention skills in both the auditory and visual domains. Joint attention is a three-part process involving engaging attention to an object of interest, shifting attention to another person, and then shifting back to the object for the purpose of socially connecting with someone about the object. Further assessment of the ability to shift attention can be gathered through additional ecologically valid approaches including observing the participant’s response to the examiner’s introduction in the waiting room, the examiner’s interruption to resume testing following a snack or break, as well as transitions between different testing tasks. Parent and teacher reports or concerns from other family members also provide valuable information about the individual’s skills for shifting attention in response to visual or auditory stimuli.
Often challenges shifting attention may be construed as rigidity, poor transitions, and/or perseveration on preferred activities. While these characteristics may apply, deficits in shifting attention are likely primordial. Specifically, it is likely that the individual will still exhibit problems shifting attention even if an adult interrupts carrying balloons or ice cream or some other highly desired object. In addition, many loving parents routinely complain that their child with ASD ignores them, and yet there is often a strong bond between the child and their nurturing parent. Recognizing weaknesses or deficits is especially important in reducing or eliminating academic problems because naïve teachers are rarely sympathetic to students who appear to ignore their instructions and carry on doing their own thing.
Recommendations for Shifting Attention
Recommendations for individuals who exhibit problems disengaging or shifting attention involve pharmacotherapy that would target ACh levels as well as behavioral interventions which primarily emphasize antecedent strategies occurring before the desired attention shift. Such antecedent strategies include priming the participant for what to expect using a visual or textual schedule, timer, or auditory warnings such as “in 5 minutes we’re going to do ___.” Since individuals with ASD are known not to shift attention efficiently, it is necessary to provide extra time for them to disengage and shift to a new activity or conversation. Individuals with ASD, including those with intellectual disabilities, may especially benefit from prompts or cues in other sensory modalities including a gentle tap on the hand or shoulder to help them shift attention. Some persons with ASD seem particularly acute in attending to music. Timing a transition from an activity with the end or beginning of a musical selection may also afford a creative approach to aiding in shifting attention.
Executive Control Network
Posner and Petersen (1989) included the executive control networks in their conceptualization of attention. Clearly, many elements of executive control including set-shifting, inhibition, and working memory are relevant in any discussion on attention. These constructs are included in the discussion on Executive Functioning.
Assessing for Executive Functioning
Vignette #8 DeShawn: Executive Functioning Assessment in an Adult with Romantic Relationship Challenges
DeShawn is a 30-year-old man previously diagnosed with depression who was referred for an evaluation for an Autism Spectrum Disorder as he was having self-reported difficulty at home. DeShawn is married and has an interest in music. He and his wife play together in a band and occasionally socialize with bandmates and other musicians in quiet settings. DeShawn relies on his wife to plan and organize their life including paying bills, shopping, and planning social and musical engagements. She feels he struggles to provide her with emotional support though he is a calm and trustworthy husband. DeShawn is a software engineer by trade and reports feeling successful at work though he keeps to himself and does not really know his coworkers well. He often needs to write things down at work to remember his daily schedule and addresses the projects one at a time (Table 11.2).
Table 11.2
Assessing for executive functioning
Associated area |
2. Executive functions |
•Inhibition: difficulty getting individuals with ASD to stop preferred activities and transition to another less preferred activity (do not inhibit their desire to focus on other activities) |
•Planning: complex cognitive skill requiring the identification and organization of steps toward a goal (often difficult for individuals with ASD) |
•Flexibility: shifting one’s response set or update cognitive strategy in response to new information |
•Working Memory: regulates attentional and cognitive control processes to permit the simultaneous storage and processing of information while performing cognitive tasks. People with ASD struggle to manipulate stimuli compared with neurotypicals, while they can adequately store visual and verbal information |
•Consider results from: BNT-2, BRIEF, CTMT, TOL-II, STROOP, PASAT, WCST, and observed ability to inhibit prepotent responses |
Full measure names |
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functioning (BRIEF) |
Boston Naming Test, Second Edition (BNT-2) |
Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) |
Tower of London, Second Edition (TOL-II) |
Stroop Color and Word Test—Child/Adult Version |
Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) |
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST ) |
DeShawn met criteria on the ADOS-2 for ASD. In light of the model presented in Chap. 7, evaluators decided to pursue additional tests beyond the Core Areas of cognitive, language, social, and sensory, based on the results of the assessment of core areas; as well as, the findings during the initial consult. Thus, examiners also assessed memory, attention, and executive functioning, based on DeShawn and his wife’s reported concerns. Memory and attention results were Average and DeShawn had High Average cognitive abilities on the WAIS-IV in Verbal and Nonverbal while Working Memory which was Low Average. DeShawn’s processing speed was Below Average. On the CTMT, Stroop, WCST, and TOL-2, DeShawn demonstrated difficulty planning, shifting, and inhibiting his responses. Overall, results of tests of executive functioning indicate deficient skills in planning, organizing, sequencing, and problem solving.

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

