Autism and the Pervasive Developmental Disorders



Autism and the Pervasive Developmental Disorders


Fred R. Volkmar

Catherine Lord

Ami Klin

Robert Schultz

Edwin H. Cook



The pervasive developmental disorders (PDDs) comprise a group of neuropsychiatric disorders characterized by specific delays and deviance in social, communicative, and cognitive development, with an onset typically in the first years of life. Although commonly associated with mental retardation, these disorders differ from other developmental disorders in that their behavioral features are distinctive and do not simply reflect developmental delay (1). Although better definitions of the syndrome continue to be needed, the validity of autism as a diagnostic category is now well established. In addition to autism, various other disorders are included with the PDD class in DSM-IV. These include Asperger’s disorder, Rett disorder, childhood disintegrative disorder (CDD), and the “subthreshold” concept of pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) (2), The validity of these other proposed PDDs has been more controversial (3). Collectively, these conditions are also often referred to as the autism spectrum disorders (ASDs).


Autistic Disorder


Definition

The DSM-IV (2) definition of autism was developed on the basis of a very large, international, multisite field trial. This definition retains historical continuity with previous definitions of autism but differs from its immediate predecessor in that age of onset (before age 3 years) is included as a necessary diagnostic feature and criteria are more conceptually framed. Diagnostic criteria are presented in Table 5.1.1.1.

The characteristic social and communicative deficits are believed to be aberrant relative to the person’s developmental level (1); consistent with Kanner’s original report and subsequent research, social difficulties in autism are a key diagnostic feature 4,5,6. Complexities for definitions of autism have included the broad range of syndrome expression, changes in syndrome expression with age, and differentiation from other psychiatric and developmental disorders 3,6.


History

Kraeplin’s description of dementia praecox was rapidly extended to children, and the terms childhood schizophrenia and childhood psychosis became synonymous. Early assumptions that all severe childhood disturbance must be a form of schizophrenia were based, in large part, on severity. Kanner’s 1943 (4) description of 11 cases of “early infantile autism” noted various ways in which this disorder appeared to be distinctive. These cases exhibited an apparently congenital inability to relate to other people (in contrast to an apparent ability and overconcern with the nonsocial environment); their language (when it developed at all) was remarkable for echolalia, pronoun reversal, and concreteness. Behaviorally, these children engaged in repetitive, apparently purposeless activities (stereotypy), and were intolerant of change. Kanner’s use of the term autism was meant to convey the unusual, self-centered quality that his cases exhibited, but it was also suggestive of the autism associated with schizophrenia. Although Kanner’s description has been remarkably enduring, early speculations about certain aspects of the condition (e.g., normal levels of intelligence, lack of association with other medical conditions, unusual levels of parental education) proved incorrect (3). The validity of autism was established over the next several decades only, as various lines of evidence became available (3). Autism was included in the landmark third edition of the DSM (DSM-III) (7). The initial definition of the condition in DSM-III lacked an appreciation of developmental change and was modified
in DSM-III-R (8). Unfortunately, while DSM-III-R was more developmentally organized, it proved problematic as it appeared that the diagnostic construct was overly broadened (3). As a result, substantial revisions were made in the DSM-IV (6). Table 5.1.1.2 summarizes historical highlights in the development of the concepts of childhood psychosis and autism.








TABLE 5.1.1.1 DSM-IV Criteria for Autistic Disorder (299.0)








  1. A total of at least six items from 1), 2), and 3), with at least two from 1), and one each from 2) and 3):


    1. Qualitative impairment in social interaction, as manifested by at least two of the following:


      1. Marked impairment in the use of multiple nonverbal behaviors such as eye-to-eye gaze, facial expression, body postures, and gestures to regulate social interaction
      2. Failure to develop peer relationships appropriate to developmental level
      3. A lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoyment, interests, or achievements with other people (e.g., by a lack of showing, bringing, or pointing out objects of interest to other people)
      4. Lack of social or emotional reciprocity

    2. Qualitative impairments in communication as manifested by at least one of the following:


      1. Delay in, or total lack of, the development of spoken language (not accompanied by an attempt to compensate through alternative modes of communication, such as gestures or mime)
      2. In individuals with adequate speech, marked impairment in the ability to initiate or sustain a conversation with others
      3. Stereotyped and repetitive use of language or idiosyncratic language
      4. Lack of varied spontaneous make-believe play or social imitative play appropriate to developmental level

    3. Restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests, and activities, as manifested by at least one of the following:


      1. Encompassing preoccupation with one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns of interest that are abnormal either in intensity or focus
      2. Apparently compulsive adherence to specific, nonfunctional routines or rituals
      3. Stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms (e.g., hand or finger flapping or twisting, or complex whole body movements)
      4. Persistent preoccupation with parts of objects

  2. Delays or abnormal functioning in at least one of the following areas, with onset prior to age three:


    1. Social interaction,
    2. Language as used in social communication, or
    3. Symbolic or imaginative play.

  3. Not better accounted for by Rett disorder or childhood disintegrative disorder.
Reprinted, with permission, from American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual (4th ed). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press, 1994.


Epidemiology

More than 40 epidemiological studies have been published; in his review, Fombonne (9) reports prevalence estimates in studies conducted since 1987 ranging from 2.5 to 72.6 per 10,000 with a median rate of 11.3 per 10,000. There does appear to be an increase in reported prevalence over time, although several factors complicate the interpretation of this observation. For example, there is more public awareness of autism, and criteria for the condition have been changed and probably effectively broadened the concept. Furthermore, the lay press and public often equate the notion of “autism spectrum disorder” with more strictly defined autism (10). Other factors accounting for discrepancies include differences in screening and ascertainment procedures, and the size of target populations (with higher rates generally reported in studies with smaller samples). Although an increase in reported rates might reflect an actual increase in incidence, other factors, such as case definition and recognition, are important. Thus, newer studies are more likely to adopt broader definitions of autism. Selected epidemiological reports are summarized in Table 5.1.1.3.








TABLE 5.1.1.2 DEVELOPMENT OF DIAGNOSTIC CONCEPTS: CHILDHOOD “PSYCHOSIS”






























Diagnostic Concept Current Terminology Originator
Dementia praecocissima Childhood schizophrenia DeSanctis, 1906
Dementia infantilis Childhood disintegrative disorder Heller, 1908
Early infantile autism Autistic disorder Kanner, 1943
Autistic psychopathy Asperger’s disorder Asperger, 1944
Atypical personality development PDD-NOS Rank, 1949
Rett syndrome Rett disorder Rett, 1966






Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Jun 8, 2016 | Posted by in PSYCHIATRY | Comments Off on Autism and the Pervasive Developmental Disorders

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access

TABLE 5.1.1.3 SELECTED EPIDEMIOLOGICAL STUDIES OF AUTISM