The clinical features of schizophrenia embrace a diverse range of disturbances of perception, thought, emotion, motivation, and motor activity. It is an illness in which episodes of florid disturbance are usually set against a background of sustained disability. The level of chronic disability ranges from a mild decrease in the ability to cope with stress, to a profound difficulty in initiating and organizing activity that can render patients unable to care for themselves.
Disorders of thought and perception
Delusions
Although there are no features that provide an unambiguous distinction between the delusions of schizophrenia and those of other psychotic illnesses, the delusions most typical of schizophrenia have an enigmatic character rarely seen in other disorders. In contrast to the delusions of affective psychosis, which usually have content consistent with the prevailing emotional state, in schizophrenia delusions often appear to reflect a fragmented experience of reality. This fragmentation is manifest in several ways.
There is a lack of logical consistency between the components of the belief, or between the belief and common understanding of what is possible. For example, a patient was very distressed by the belief that he had no head and also that there was blood all over his face.
Behaviour bears an unpredictable relationship to the delusional belief. In some instances, the patient believes he has a special role or identity, yet for the most part, lives a life that is scarcely influenced by the belief. In the words of Bleuler(1): ‘Kings, Emperors, Popes, and Redeemers engage for the most part, in quite banal work, provided they still have any energy at all for activity’.
In the chronic phase of the illness, patients might acknowledge that a former delusion was not justified, yet in the same interview they reiterate the delusional belief. Bleuler(1) reported: ‘sometimes the patients even produce thoughts which are only understandable if it is assumed that the delusions still retain some reality for these patients even though consciously they may reject them’.
The mental mechanism of schizophrenic delusions remains to be ascertained. It is not a lack of capacity for logical thought; rather it appears that certain ideas acquire an attribute that exempts them from the normal processes of validation. This phenomenon is illustrated by the historic case of Daniel Schreber,(2) a high-ranking judge from Leipzig, who suffered a late-onset schizophrenic illness. After obtaining a court order for discharge from his second hospital admission he published his memoirs(3) in a volume that includes his own account of his beliefs, and also the report prepared by the asylum director, Dr Weber, opposing his discharge. For the purpose of understanding the nature of delusions in schizophrenia, Schreber’s account is of special value because we have access to his own perceptions of his condition in addition to detailed accounts by his physician. Dr Weber reported that Schreber exhibited lively interest in his social environment, a well-informed mind, and sound judgement, while nonetheless maintaining his delusional beliefs in a manner that would accept no contrary argument. Schreber himself agreed that his beliefs were unchangeable. He believed that he had a mission to redeem the world and restore humankind to its lost state of bliss. His system of delusions included the belief that he was being transformed into a voluptuous female partner of God. He considered that his beliefs belonged to a domain that was exempt from normal logic: ‘I could even say with Jesus Christ: My kingdom is not of this world; my so-called delusions are concerned solely with God and the beyond’. Furthermore, he maintained total conviction in his core beliefs despite recognizing that his experiences earlier in his illness had been unrealistic. He stated:
Having lived for months among miracles, I was inclined to take more or less everything I saw for a miracle. Accordingly, I did not know whether to take the streets of Leipzig through which I traveled as only theatre props, perhaps in the fashion in which Prince Potemkin is said to have put them up for Empress Catherine II of Russia during her travels through the desolate country, so as to give the impression of a flourishing countryside.
Thus, in the stable phase of his illness, Schreber recognized that his earlier experiences were unrealistic and that his current beliefs defied normal logic, but appeared to regard them as exempt from the need for validation. The late onset of his illness and his high level of professional achievement are unusual for an individual with schizophrenia, and raise questions about the diagnosis. However, the fact that he eventually suffered a marked deterioration in function during his third episode of illness strongly supports the diagnosis of schizophrenia.
In many instances, the delusions of schizophrenia appear to arise from an altered experience of self. The phenomena identified by the German psychiatrist, Kurt Schneider(4) as first-rank symptoms of schizophrenia (discussed in greater detail below) include several symptoms that entail an aberrant experience of ownership of one’s own thought, will, action, emotion, or bodily function, which the patient attributes to alien influence. In some cases, delusions might arise from a delusional mood, i.e. an altered sense of reality in which the current circumstances acquire an indefinable transcendental quality.
Although the delusions most characteristic of schizophrenia have an incongruous quality, it is not uncommon for schizophrenic patients to have coherent delusions that are internally consistent and produce predictable behavioural responses. In particular, coherent persecutory delusions are common, and can lead to defensive actions such barricading oneself in one’s room with blinds drawn. Ideas of reference and delusions of reference are also prevalent. For example, a patient might report that television programmes refer specifically to him or her. In the International Pilot Study of Schizophrenia(5) conducted by the World Health Organization, ideas of reference were reported in 70 per cent of cases, suspiciousness in 66 per cent, and delusions of persecution in 64 per cent.
Hallucinations
Hallucinations in any modality can occur, but auditory hallucinations are the most prevalent in schizophrenia. Hearing voices speaking in the third person is the most specific. This experience is listed among the Schneiderian first-rank symptoms. Sometimes the content is mundane, as in the instance when a patient of Bleuler(1) heard a voice saying ‘Now she is combing her hair’ while she was grooming in the morning. In other instances there is an implied criticism, as in the case reported by Schneider(4) of a woman who heard a voice saying ‘Now she is eating; here she is munching again’, whenever she wanted to eat.
Second-person auditory hallucinations are also common. In the International Pilot Study of Schizophrenia,(5) voices speaking to the patient were reported in 65 per cent of cases. Voices might issue commands that the patient obeys. In some instances, the patient engages in a dialogue with the voices.
During the acute phase of illness, auditory hallucinations usually have the same sensory quality as voices arising from sources in the external world. In some instances the voice is attributed to a radio-transmitter implanted in the body, especially in the teeth. In the chronic phase, the voices are often recognized as coming from within the person’s own mind. Kraepelin(6) reports: ‘at other times they do not appear to the patient as sense perceptions at all; they are ‘voices of conscience’; ‘voices which do not speak with words”. These experiences are pseudohallucinations, but nonetheless they are a significant feature in many cases.
In schizophrenia, visual hallucinations are less common than auditory hallucinations, but do occur. Somatic hallucinations are also relatively common, and often are associated with a delusional misinterpretation. For example, a young man reported sensations in his belly that he attributed to a snake, which he believed had crawled up his anus.
Schneiderian first-rank symptoms
Kurt Schneider(4) identified a set of phenomena that he considered were strongly indicative of schizophrenia in the absence of overt brain disease. These symptoms, listed in Table 4.3.2.1, have become known as first-rank symptoms. Schneider did not consider that the diagnosis could be made simply on the presence of one such symptom; on the contrary, he warned,(4) ‘a psychotic phenomenon is not like a defective stone in an otherwise perfect mosaic’. Schneider did not define the phenomena precisely, and clinicians have interpreted his writings differently. Mellor(7) formulated a precise set of definitions and found that, according to these strict criteria, 72 per cent of patients with schizophrenia exhibited at least one first-rank symptom. Applying the same criteria, O’Grady(8) found that in a series of cases assessed at admission to hospital, 73 per cent of schizophrenic patients exhibited at least one first-rank symptom, while no cases of affective psychosis did. However, applying less strict criteria, O’Grady found more broadly defined first-rank symptoms in 14 per cent of patients with affective psychosis.
Table 4.3.2.1 Schneiderian first-rank symptoms
Voices commenting—a hallucinatory voice commenting on one’s actions in the third person
Voices discussing or arguing—hallucinations of two or more voices discussing or arguing about oneself
Audible thought—hearing one’s thoughts aloud
Thought insertion—the insertion, by an alien sources, of thoughts that are experienced as not being one’s own
Thought withdrawal—the withdrawal of thoughts from one’s mind by an alien agency
Thought broadcast—the experience that one’s thoughts are broadcast so as to be accessible to others
Made will—the experience of one’s will being controlled by an alien influence
Made acts—the experience that acts executed by one’s own body are the actions of an alien agency, rather than oneself
Made affect—the experience of emotion that is not one’s own, attributed to an alien influence
Somatic passivity—bodily function is controlled by an alien influence
Delusional perception—the attribution of a totally unwarranted meaning to a normal perception
Three of the first-rank symptoms (voices commenting, voices discussing, and audible thoughts) involve auditory hallucinations, while the remainder entail delusional attributions to experiences or perceptions. Although Schneider himself avoided speculating on the theoretical implications of these phenomena, it is notable that most of them involve a disorder of the sense of ownership of one’s own mental or physical activity. Thought broadcast, thought withdrawal, and thought insertion reflect the experience of loss of autonomy over thought, while made will, made acts, made affect, and somatic passivity reflect loss of autonomy over action, will, affect, and bodily function.
Mellor(7) emphasizes that there are two aspects to these phenomena: the experience of loss of autonomy and the delusional attribution to alien influence. As an illustration of made acts, Mellor reports a patient who reported that his fingers moved to pick up objects ‘but I don’t control them … I sit there watching them move, and they are quite independent, what they do is nothing to do with me. I am just a puppet … I am just a puppet who is manipulated by cosmic strings’. To illustrate made affect, Mellor quotes a young woman: ‘I cry, tears roll down my cheeks and I look unhappy, but inside I have a cold anger because they are using me in this way, and it is not me who is unhappy, but they are projecting unhappiness into my brain’.
Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue