Diagnostic Assessment

and Gordon N. Dutton2



(1)
Department of Psychology, LMU Munich, München, Germany

(2)
Department of Visual Science, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK

 




6.1 Some Preliminary Notes


Diagnostic assessment of CVI in children is an interdisciplinary task, which needs close cooperation between the disciplines of (neuro-)ophthalmology, neuropaediatrics, developmental neuropsychology, orthoptics as well as early intervention and special education specialists. Observations reported by parents, brothers and sisters and other reference persons about the child’s visually guided behaviour such as grasping for toys, visual exploration, fixation, responses to display of toys and other objects and play behaviour can provide significant indications concerning spared and affected visual capacities. In principle, it has to be acknowledged that when assessing children, who do not possess sufficient language competence and, therefore, cannot report or comment on what they can see and why vision is problematic, behaviour-based methods are used. Even for older children with CVI, it might be difficult to report in detail what they can see and can make out, in terms of useful vision. Language development may be delayed, and naming may be impaired because of semantic problems or because visual accuracy is (was) not sufficient to learn reliable associations between visual stimuli and their labels. Delayed language development may also make understanding of instruction and giving the proper verbal response difficult for the child. In these cases, behaviour-based assessment methods should be given preference. The reliable assessment of visual capacities, and their impairments, respectively, supposes at least sufficient cognitive, in particular attention, motor capacities and motivation, including visual curiosity. It is important to note that responses to visual stimuli, or performance in a visual task, which cannot be replicated, should not be taken for granted as contributing to a diagnosis.

Apart from the conventional criteria for tests, validity, reliability and objectivity, the so-called ecological validity plays an essential role in both the diagnostic assessment and treatment of functional impairments. Ecological validity comprises three dimensions, the nature of the setting, stimuli and response, and signifies (1) the degree to which the observed and recorded behaviour/performance reflects the behaviour/performance that a subject shows/possesses in the ‘real world and (2) the degree to which findings based on standardised test instruments can be generalised (or extended) to natural, everyday life settings (Schmuckler 2001). Of course, ecological validity varies between functional domains and thus assessment instruments. The diagnosis of homonymous hemianopia, for example, possesses a rather low ecological validity, because it does not include any information concerning the degree of compensation and thus the associated visual impairment. In contrast, impaired visual search performance will allow a good prognosis concerning a child’s behaviour in everyday life conditions, because reduced accuracy tells us that the child will omit targets and reduced speed indicates that the child needs much more time for the visual search process, with limited search time also resulting in neglecting parts of the actual scene.

For the acquisition of valid, significant and reliable diagnostic data as well as for comparisons in follow-up (longitudinal) assessments of the same child and between children, it is essential to keep testing conditions comparable, i.e. to assess vision in standard conditions. Standardisation includes lighting conditions, equipment of the testing room (friendly atmosphere, but not too many pictures and furniture, because the child may be distracted), type and duration of stimulus presentation and verbal and nonverbal interaction with the child. Any effective means of stimulation of the child’s motivation by the investigator is welcome, but this may also be too much for the child to cope with, due to information overload with visual, auditory and tactile stimuli; therefore, ‘less is sometimes more’. In addition, it is important to consider breaks as often and as long as required. New visual stimuli may increase attention and enhance motivation; it seems sensible, therefore, not to show the same stimulus too often successively, because the child may show habituation to this stimulus and thus fail to respond (Fantz 1964). Results, which are obtained under conditions that do not guarantee examination, can be used for preliminary screening, but not for diagnostic reports.

The examination of the various functional capacities of the central visual system should always seek not only impaired visual capacities but also those that are spared. The so-called positive and negative picture of performance is not only important to understand the quality and degree of visual disability but also for the planning of therapeutic interventions and the benefit of these interventions. The use of standardised assessment tools is, therefore, recommended; in addition, data from children with normal development of the same age, gender and socio-economic status should be available for comparison.

There is still a lack of standardised diagnostic tools. Many assessment measures that are used in developmental research cannot easily be translated into diagnostic practice; on the other hand, proven tests, for example, to assess visual acuity or contrast vision, are not very helpful for the evaluation of (the degree of) visual disability in individual everyday life conditions of the child, i.e. ecological validity is rather low (Colenbrander 2009, 2010; Boot et al. 2010; see Sect. 7.​3) when other visual disabilities are evident. The verification of spared manifest visual capacities often meets with difficulties, because assessment standards designed for typical children may have limited salience for the child whose visual system has developed differently. Here, a quasi-experimental procedure may be helpful: one factor is manipulated in a systematic and reproducible way, for example, the use of coloured instead of ‘black’ symbols and forms may be used to estimate visual acuity, because coloured stimuli can have higher salience for the specific child (exceptions: congenital colour deficiencies or cerebral dyschromatopsia). Visual acuity can also be estimated by means of moving stimuli that differ in size, because accuracy of smooth pursuit eye movements depends crucially on visually acuity (Atkinson and Braddick 1979; see Sect. 6.5.4). This quasi-experimental approach may be especially helpful if ‘normal’ assessment procedures do not produce useful results. Such deviations from the standardised assessment should, however, always be documented. In addition, it should be clearly understood that such procedures do not possess the same diagnostic quality in terms of validity, although reliability and objectivity can, however, be achieved by using this type of approach in a standardised form. Diagnostic assessment tools presented in this chapter, which have been standardised, accord with the outcome from developmental studies in children with normal development (e.g. Granrud 1993; Slater 1998a, b). Finally, it should be added that measurements of visual functions and capacities should be repeated by the examiner, and in doubtful cases by another examiner; this can increase the reliability of measurements considerably and thus also the validity of the test used (Mash and Dobson 2005).


6.2 The Method of Preferential Looking (PL)


The method of PL was developed by Fantz and Oddy (1959) to assess visual capacities at the behavioural level in babies and young children. The method is based on the observation that newborn infants show ‘natural’ preferences for particular visual stimuli, i.e. they favour such stimuli above others and, therefore, fixate upon them longer (see Sect. 2.​3.​6). This preference behaviour underlies many assessment methods in visual development, for example, for visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, discrimination of colour hues and of forms, figures and complex visual stimuli and size and number of stimuli (Miranda 1970; Fantz and Fagan 1975; Clavadetscher et al. 1988). Estimation of the corresponding visual capacity is based on fixation times, whereby the particular stimulus of a stimulus pair (e.g. that has been preferred by at least 75 % of normal children) is taken as the cut-off score for discrimination (Fantz 1964). It is, however, important to avoid habituation effects by presenting the same stimulus pair too often.

The method of PL has been proven reliable for the assessment of visual acuity in children with CVI (Chandna et al. 1989; Birch and Bane 1991; Schmidt 1994). It is, however, important to mention that the behavioural response in question must be attainable by the child. Table 6.1 summarises typical visual activities of children aged 1–12 months, which may also serve as a guideline for diagnostic assessment in children with CVI. For determination of the so-called difference threshold, pairs of stimuli are shown, whereby one stimulus remains unchanged (so-called standard stimulus), while the other stimulus is approximated (variable stimulus), so that the difference between the two stimuli decreases progressively. If no preference for either stimulus can be observed, i.e. the frequency of orienting responses, and duration of fixations are more or less equal, the limit of stimulus discrimination has been reached, and this value can be taken as the difference threshold. To avoid too high a demand on visual memory, the time between pair presentation should not be longer than 3–5 s. Apart from direct observation of oculomotor and grasping responses, video recordings of responses may be very helpful, because they allow repeated analysis and are, in addition, very suitable for individual longitudinal assessments, as well as for comparisons between children (e.g. children with different manifestations of CVI, or children with CVI and those with normal visual development). Eye movements to visual stimuli can also be recorded using video- and infrared-registration techniques. Further sources for getting information about visual capacities include the systematic observation of visually guided everyday life activities and the use of electrophysiological methods, which allow recording of physiological correlations of visual capacities (Lundh 1989; Hartmann et al. 1990; Bane and Birch 1992; Bravarone et al. 1993; Katsumi et al. 1995, 1997, 1998; Lewis et al. 1995; Rydberg and Ericson 1998).


Table 6.1
Typical visual activities during the first 12 months in children with normal development



























































Months 0–1

Looks at light stimuli, turns eyes and head

Blink responses as avoidance behaviour to bright light

Eye contact with others

Slow and jerky horizontal pursuit eye movements

Months 2–3

Intensive eye contact

Interest in mobiles

Vertical pursuit eye movements

Months 4–6

Grasps at moving objects

Observes falling and rolling away of objects

Shifts fixation across midline

Extends visual field of search (attention)

Begins to decouple eye from head movements

Pursuit eye movements are smooth

Months 7–10

Becomes aware of and detects small objects

Touches and later grasps at stationary objects

Is interested in pictures and scenes

Keeps eye contact with others over a distance of several metres

Months 11–12

Good visual orientation in familiar surroundings

Looks through window and recognises familiar persons

Can visually recognise pictures; plays hide and seek

Observes and examines objects in detail

Communicates visually in an efficient way (understands and uses facial expressions and gestures)


Modified after Hyvarinen (2000); see also Table 2.​14


6.3 Visual Evoked Potentials (VEPs)


Registration of VEPs facilitates determination of the quality and speed of processing of visual stimuli, e.g. patterned black-and-white stimuli, which are transmitted from the retina to the primary visual cortical area (striate cortex, Brodmann area 17, V1), where the electrical responses are generated (Barnikol et al. 2006). Latency and amplitude of the first positive wave (P1) are the most commonly used parameters to evaluate the electrophysiological responses. However, it must be recognised that the amplitude and form of the VEP depend crucially on fixation accuracy and concentration upon the stimulus pattern during the recording, because the major contribution to the amplitude of P1 stems from the central visual field (Cannon 1983). Furthermore, even if the form, amplitude and latency of P1 are optimal for analysis, a fundamental problem remains. These parameters characterise the electrophysiological response to a defined visual stimulus, which means that it is a kind of correlate of vision, but is neither a measure of true vision nor an assessment of visual capacity, e.g. visual acuity or contrast sensitivity (Arroyo et al. 1997). Fortunately, there exist highly positive correlations in children (and adults) between the amplitude of P1 responses to a pattern reversal checkerboard or grating stimulus and visual acuity and spatial contrast sensitivity, respectively, if an adequate range of spatial frequencies is used (0.1–30 cycles/degree; Ridder 2004; Lenassi et al. 2008; Mackay et al. 2008; Iyer et al. 2013). Pattern-evoked visual potentials have also been proved to possess high sensitivity and specificity in children with suspected functional visual acuity loss (Hamilton et al. 2013), in preterm children with subtle visual dysfunction (O’Reilly et al. 2010) and in children with spastic cerebral palsy (Costa and Ventura 2012). In addition, pattern-evoked visual potentials may also be useful in longitudinal assessment of visual function in children with CVI (Watson et al. 2007), but besides parallel courses of PL, visual acuity and VEP development, disparities in PL and VEP can also be found (Lim et al. 2005). Unfortunately, however, a sufficient quality of P1 can occur in the context of cerebral blindness. Nearly normal P1 responses after pattern stimulation have been recorded in adults with bilateral destruction of V1 and resulting severe visual disability, characterised by a visual acuity 1.3logMAR (<0.05). Apparently, a smaller number of spared neurons in V1 is required for generating P1 than are needed to ‘see’ the same stimulus pattern and discriminate pattern components (Celesia et al. 1982; Wygnanksi-Jaffe et al. 2009). Thus, the preserved P1 does not always allow a valid conclusion to be drawn about visual acuity or contrast vision; the only correct conclusion that can be drawn is that there is good transmission of visually triggered retinal signals to V1 and that there is still some neuronal activity in this visual cortical area, which taken positively could provide evidence for persisting cortical visual function. Despite the methodological and interpretational difficulties, VEP recordings may be useful in children with severe forms of CVI (van Genderen et al. 2006). In some cases, it is the only way to get diagnostic information about the morphological and physiological ‘state’ of the visual system (Taylor and McCulloch 1992), which in addition, allows a prognosis concerning the development of visual acuity in preterm children and in children with CVI (Atkinson et al. 2002a; Watson et al. 2010). Measurements can also be used as a guide to the dimensions of habilitational and educational materials to evaluate for responses and use. However, VEP vernier acuity appears more affected in subjects with CVI and is more associated with behavioural measures of grating acuity than is VEP grating acuity (Skoczenski and Good 2004; Watson et al. 2009). It should be added here that flash-evoked visual potentials are not useful to assess cortical visual function, because they are mainly generated by subcortical structures of the visual system and thus do not reflect acuity-equivalent visual processing (Schroeder et al. 1989).


6.4 Diagnostic Procedures in Children with CVI


In this section, assessment methods are presented for visual and oculomotor functions and capacities, which play a prominent role in visual perception and its development and which can be assessed with high validity and reliability: visual field, spatial contrast sensitivity and visual acuity, colour and form discrimination, localisation of visual stimuli, stereopsis, object and face perception and oculomotor functions including visual exploration and visual search. Thus, a comprehensive set of visual and oculomotor tools should not solely comprise visual field, visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and eye movements, although such a tool set can serve as a valid basic standard with high availability and simple use by practitioners (Powers 2009). Emphasis is on children in the first 3–4 years; for older children, assessment standards exist or neuropsychological assessment methods can be applied, which are also used with young adults (see Fischer and Cole 2000; Hyvarinen 2000; Topor and Erin 2000; Zihl 2011; Lezak et al. 2012). In older children who show severe multiple disabilities, particularly in cognition, the tests described can also be used. It should be mentioned that not all tests are sufficiently standardised and for some tests data from normal children for comparison are missing.

In cases of suspected CVI, it appears reasonable and useful to follow a standard scheme. After making the first contact with the child, including systematic observation of the child’s behaviour (which often gives the first indication of visual difficulties), the medical history, head and body posture, eyelids, poisition of the eyes and possible spontaneous eye movements (e.g. spontaneous nystagmus and gaze dependant nystagmus) are elicited. After checking the ability of the child to participate sufficiently in the assessment, anterior parts of the eye, pupil function, accommodation, vergence, ocular motility (saccades and pursuit eye movements) and eye, head and grasping movements to a simple visual stimulus are examined. Systematic assessment of visual and oculomotor functions and capacities is then performed. Figure 6.1 shows the various diagnostic steps in the visual and oculomotor assessment in suspected CVI in a schematic diagram; the proposed diagnostic procedure is also intended to help with differentiating primary and secondary visual and oculomotor dysfunction.

A191809_1_En_6_Fig1_HTML.gif


Fig. 6.1
Schematic diagram of diagnostic steps in children with suspected visual impairments of peripheral and central origin (so-called primary visual disorders) or due to oculomotor or nonvisual disorders (so-called secondary visual impairments)


6.4.1 History Taking to Assist Diagnosis and Guide Management of CVI


History taking comprising systematic questioning of parents or other reference persons (e.g. grandparents, mother’s help, kindergarten teachers), who can give information about the child, concerning development, in general, and visual development, in particular, but also about habits, preferences, difficulties and avoidance responses, and conditions of occurrence, is the first phase of diagnostics. Visual history taking should include questions on the child’s visually guided behaviour in everyday life situations and play behaviour but also visual curiosity, attention to visual (and other) stimuli, dealing with other children (social behaviour), etc. For the older child who can communicate well, history taking in the absence of parents is indicated. More valid and reliable information can be gained if a standardised questionnaire is used, which addresses visual performance and behaviour in everyday life conditions of the child. McCulloch et al. (2007) have used an inventory with 16 questions about visual skills and behaviour in familiar conditions to comprehensively assess 76 children (age, 7 months to 16 years) with marked neurological impairments. Visual skills and visual behaviour were rated by carers. In addition, structured clinical histories were taken, and visual acuity was measured using VEP acuity and acuity cards. Acuity ranged from mere light perception to normal and was positively associated with carer’s responses and reports. Two independent factors were found, which were significantly correlated with the outcome of visual assessment. Factor 1 was visual recognition and was correlated with visual acuity; factor 2 was visually dependent social interactions. The authors eventually identified items with the highest validity for predicting visual skills and visual behaviour (see Table 6.2). Interestingly, overall correlations between item frequencies with acuity card measures were considerably higher than with VEP acuity. Ortibus et al. (2011) investigated the utility of a CVI questionnaire as a screening instrument in children with and without CVI (mean age, 6 years) who in addition underwent a detailed visual assessment including visual perceptual and visuomotor tests. The questionnaire consists of 46 items; covers visual attention, visual fixation, visual field and the influence of a familiar environment; and is filled out by parents. Sensitivity ranges from 33.3 to 76.7 % and specificity from 70.6 to 97.1 %; thus, the questionnaire has good predictive value for the identification of children with CVI. Ferziger et al. (2011) evaluated an interdisciplinary functional questionnaire for children with cerebral palsy and intellectual disability. The questionnaire proved valid and reliable information about daily visual performance and predicted visual function with respect to task-orientated visual function and basic visual skills and may be a useful instrument for visual assessment in children with CP. Although it appears obvious to ask parents and carers to fill out visual function questionnaires, because they can respond in a standardised form, it seems sensible to also question the children with CVI in a semi-standardised form to obtain more personal and individual information regarding their difficulties in various everyday life situations and how they cope with them, provided the child possesses sufficient linguistic and intellectual capability.


Table 6.2
The visual skills inventory of McCulloch et al. (2007)







































Q1

Is your child aware of a spoonful or food coming towards his/her mouth?

Q2

Does your child reach for a small/large silent object?

Q3

Does your child’s vision seem better vision in bright light/dim light?

Q4

Does your child reach for a drink bottle?

Q5

Does your child see a small/large silent object?

Q6

Does your child reach for a small/large noisy object?

Q7

Does your child react to you approaching him without sound clues?

Q8

Does your child follow your movements around a room when you have given him no sound clues?

Q9

Does your child return your smile when you smile without any sound?

Q10

Is your child aware of himself in a mirror; if yes: 6 ft (1.8 m), 4 ft (1.4 m), 2 ft (0.7 m), 1 ft (0.35 m) or less?

Q11

Does your child screw up his eyes when taken into bright sunlight?


Q question

More detailed structured history taking using an inventory of questions, rather than a questionnaire, where clarification and more detail is sought as required for positive responses, can also be applied, particularly to move on from diagnosis to characterisation of the behavioural impact of the visual disability. One such recently validated inventory of 51 questions (Dutton et al. 2010; Macintyre-Béon et al. 2012) has proven effective in characterising the visual behaviours in children born prematurely (Macintyre-Béon et al. 2013).

The diagnosis of a number of visual conditions can only be made by history taking. Examples include the subjective symptoms of visual migraine (the fortification spectrum), amaurosis fugax (intermittent fleeting loss of vision) or Charles Bonnet syndrome (formed visual hallucinations as a sequel to loss of vision). Similarly, many of the adaptive and reactive behaviours seen in children with CVI can only be identified by taking a structured history, because they are features of everyday living, or they are no longer manifest but informative, or they comprise the older child’s subjective experiences, none of which are evident on assessment.

This section describes history taking methods, using both open and closed question sets, designed to contribute to the dual objectives of working towards a diagnosis and optimally managing each child with CVI. History taking precedes and can thus be used to guide assessment for cooperative older children. However, assessment of vision needs the child’s cooperation. For younger children, it is often best to ‘capture the moment’ and assess vision and eye movements before detailed history taking, which can then be carried out while the child is playing or being looked after.


6.4.1.1 Interview Method


Ideally the room for the interview is clean, tidy and uncluttered and has wheelchair access, space for a number of people and facilities for play. (Watching the child at play can be very informative.) The range of equipment and resources needed to fully assess vision in children of all ages ideally needs to be available.

The child and accompanying adults are welcomed and introductions made. The interviewer uses a calm voice, is kind and friendly and involves the child. The older child is made to feel important and is invited to contribute, on occasion without parents present for some of the time. Siblings and grandparents if present are also involved and can in many cases contribute useful information.

The way a question is asked can influence the answer. The question ‘What are your concerns about John’s vision?’ is open and does not lead. The question ‘What happens when you take John into a supermarket?’ is a closed but non-leading question, while the question ‘Does David become upset when you take him into a supermarket?’ is both a closed and a leading question, as the expected answer is implied. Each type of question has its place. Open, non-leading questions are used to elicit the story for the first time and to contribute to the diagnosis. Closed, non-leading questions are used to find out more about issues raised. Closed, leading questions potentially providing expected answers can be helpful later in the interview, when working out how best to help the child once the diagnostic picture has been elicited using the open, non-leading approach. Closed leading questions can help parents recognise the behavioural features being sought, as it may not have occurred to them that behaviours they see each day are in any way unusual, meaning that this information may not otherwise be elicited. Such questions can lead to their realisation that behaviours that they deemed ‘normal’ are in fact reactions or adaptive strategies to circumvent visual disorders. (However, such leading questions must be used advisedly, and responses interpreted carefully, because a small minority of families can, in our experience, take on board and ‘file’ this information to use inappropriately at a later stage to potentially enhance the story for perceived gain. Such an outcome is of course counterproductive for all parties.)

The approach we recommend comprises:

Initial Administration



  • Ascertaining the previous medical history from the referral letter and case notes.


Taking the History



  • Seeking the story


  • Clarifying the story


  • Extending the story


  • Asking open expert questions to seek diagnostic patterns of visual behaviour


  • Using an open question inventory, if required, to seek the full range of the visual difficulties and their impact


  • Selecting and employing supplementary question sets as required from a closed question inventory


  • Finding out what the family has already been told and understands


  • Understanding the wishes, agenda and aspirations of the child and family


  • Finding out about the day-to-day practical and psychological impacts the visual difficulties are having upon the child and family (to work towards identifying and implementing ways of ameliorating any adverse impact)


Assimilating the Information



  • Identifying typical clusters of features elicited to cross validate the responses and make sense of the information to contribute to the diagnosis and its pattern


  • Assembling and documenting the story and drawing conclusions


  • Using the information gathered by history taking and examination, to guide further tailored assessments if required


  • Documenting all the features elicited to compile a prioritised tailored comprehensive list of interventional strategies to use at home, at school and elsewhere


Communicating the Diagnosis and Management Plan



  • Selecting the initial strategies to work on


  • Writing a salient comprehensive report that all can understand and planning the approach to management


Initial Administration

Ascertaining the previous medical history from the referral letter and case notes. It is very helpful to have appraised and mastered all the available information before the child and accompanying adults enter the room. This gives the family confidence, and assists in guiding the strategies and pathway of history taking and assessment.


Taking the History

Seeking the story. For some children, the interview will be their first, and parents will want their observations of how their child behaves explained. For others, there may have been a number of previous encounters with a range of professionals who may have given different explanations of the findings and who may have provided a range of interpretations. Before giving any explanations, or making any plans, it is wise to ask the question ‘What have you been told already?’ followed by ‘What do you understand about your child’s vision?’ The answers to these questions are crucial to knowing how to later go about explaining what has been identified and what needs to be done. If an explanation is given which is contrary to the parents’/caregivers’ prior beliefs or explanations, this has the potential to cause difficulties.

The first question is open. For example, ask ‘What would you like to get out of this interview?’ and then listen without interruption. Parents may have a pre-planned agenda. They need to be given the opportunity to express it. If they are interrupted, this can break their train of thought, and useful information can be lost. (Parents can be invited in advance to prepare a list of questions if they wish, because material can otherwise be omitted when there is a lot to communicate.) A note is made of each key issue. Doing so in the form of a mind-map can prove helpful, as this can then be embellished as each issue is developed.

Clarifying the story. The initial history is clarified and developed in terms of ‘time, place and person’, as appropriate. For example, John becomes distressed in busy shopping centres.

Time

When did he first show this behaviour? When he was a toddler.

How often does it happen? Whenever we go to the supermarket. We don’t take him now.

How long does it last for when it happens? The whole time he’s in the shop, and for a while after until he calms down.

Has it happened recently? If so, what happened? Yes, last month during Christmas shopping. He had to go back and sit quietly in the car with his dad.

Place

Where does this happen? Can you give any examples? Yes. He went to a party. We took him early when it was quiet, but as soon as the games began and the boys ran around, he hid in a corner and wouldn’t come out.

Person

What is his behaviour like? It varies. Sometimes he becomes withdrawn like at the party; other times he can get distressed and angry.

Is there anything that makes it better? He’s great when he is out walking in the countryside. We never see this behaviour then.

Is there anything that makes it worse? Additional noise. He will stand crying in the corner with his fingers in his ears.

Does it upset David when it happens? Yes. He’s 7 years old. He doesn’t want to be different, but he’s beginning to understand that he is, and a child in his class is beginning to bully him.

Extending the story. To comprehensively extend the history a number of key issues need to be remembered. These can be considered in the following sets of three.

Asking specific questions to seek patterns of visual behaviour. Visual behaviours due to cerebral visual impairment occur in recognisable patterns that tend to occur in the triplets outlined in the box below. When one element of the triplet is affected, then evidence of dysfunction of the other two needs to be sought by salient questioning. These features may need to be specifically sought by closed history taking as they may have been considered the child’s ‘normal’ behaviour. They may, for example, have been attributed to inappropriate behaviour or to lack of intelligence and may not have been recognised as being related to disorders of vision. In some cases the visual behaviours may have been labelled as atypical autistic spectrum disorder, without recognition of the visual contribution.


Box: Issues to Consider































Each dysfunction can impact upon:

Access to information (both distant and near)

Social interaction

Visual guidance of movement of the arms, legs and body

Adaptations/reactions the child makes include:

Adaptive behaviours that make best use of intact vision

Reactive behaviours manifesting, for example, in anger or distress

Avoidance of difficult situations

Psychological impacts include:

Emotional: feelings, emotions, anxieties

Interpersonal: interaction with other children

Fatigue: due to the effort needed to utilise vision

Structured history taking considers the impact of how the measured visual functions such as visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and visual field predictably impact upon the three domains of social interaction, access to information and mobility. Additional question inventories act as a prompt to ensure full history taking, in which issues raised are clarified as required, and additional questions may be asked. This is essential for children with CVI as each child’s condition is unique.

Using an open question inventory, if required, to seek the full range of the visual difficulties and their impact. Two question inventories are presented below. Table 6.3 presents a set of closed non-leading questions aimed at helping older children, parents and caregivers to describe behaviours that are commonly described (Ahmed and Dutton 1996; Dutton et al. 1996, 2004, 2006; Gillen and Dutton 2003; Drummond and Dutton 2007; Saidkasimova et al. 2007). It seeks evidence of typical adaptive and reactive behaviours seen in children with perceptual and cognitive visual dysfunction and addresses parents’ perceptions of the child’s emotional development, as well as their hopes and aspirations for their child’s future.


Table 6.3
An inventory of specific, targeted, non-leading questions that seek typical behaviours commonly seen in children with a diagnosis of CVI

































































































































































































































































































































































































Non-leading question

Anticipated answer

Possible explanation

Visual field/attentional impairment

How well does your child…

Clear food from his/her plate?

Food tends to be left on the right/left/near side of the plate

Food consistently left on the right, left or near side of the plate indicates possible visual inattention or visual field impairment in the area indicated

Cope with reading?

Tends to miss the start of a line. Tends to miss the next word. Misses text at the bottom of the page

Suggests left or right acquired hemianopia, or lower visual field impairment

Cross roads?

Misses traffic coming from the right/left

Suggests right or left hemianopia or inattention

Cope with going through doorways?

Consistently bumps into one side

Suggests right- or left-sided visual inattention. (left more common)

Does your child…
   

Do anything to help ensure that food on the plate is not missed?

The plate is rotated to find food that has been missed

This is a strategy children often use to compensate for a visual field or attentional deficit

Sit at table in any particular way?

The body is rotated slightly

This suggests possible left- or right-sided inattention

Lower visual field impairment

How does your child…

Walk along with you?

Holds onto my clothes, or pocket or belt (pulling down)

Gains advance tactile guidance to ground height

Behave when a coffee table has been moved?

Can bump into it and become angry that the table has been moved

The table is inadequately seen in the lower visual field, and prior mapping of its location has been disturbed

Go down a slide?

Goes down on stomach

Wants to use the upper visual field to see down the slide

Refuses to go down sitting

Absent lower visual field makes it frightening to go down while sitting

Find shoes?

Shoes on the ground cannot be found

Shoes not seen in absent lower visual field

Put on shoes?

Either lifts the foot up onto a step or lies on back with foot in the air

Foot has to be raised so as to see it

Jump off a wall or a bench?

Frightened to do so

Fear of landing because ground cannot be seen.

Jump into a swimming pool?

Frightened to do so

Fear of not knowing height to jump and possibly landing on someone

Go down stairs?

Going down by sitting on the steps or holding the banister with both hands

Cannot see the steps

Negotiate pavement kerbs?

Walks off steps by mistake. Trips over kerbs. Lifts foot too early/late, high/low

Kerbs may be incompletely seen or not seen at all

Deal with low obstacles?

Collision is frequent

Low obstacles are not seen

Go from one place to another?

Runs everywhere (and tends to trip often)

Most children with lower visual field impairment tend to run frequently, perhaps to get to the more distant visible ground ahead, while its location is registered

Go up and down slopes?

Slopes are much easier to walk up than down. The child can often ‘get stuck’ at the top

The ground ahead on downward slopes may not be visible unless the head it tipped down, but this can interfere with balance

Watch TV?

Lies on the floor watching it upside down

Using intact upper visual field

Cross floor boundaries?

Probes the floor boundary with the foot before crossing it

Floor boundaries and steps may be difficult to distinguish due to low contrast, low acuity or lower visual field impairment

Play with wheeled toys?

Wheeled toys are very popular and used all the time (by younger children)

Such toys give a tactile guide to the height of the ground ahead and (by bumping into them) the locations of obstacles

Cope with obstacles on the floor?

They are tripped over

Because they are not seen

Clear food from his/her plate?

Food is left on the near side of the plate

Because it is not seen, suggesting the potential for superadded inattention in the lower visual field

Fill a cup or glass with water?

This can be very difficult

Because it is in the lower visual field and of low contrast

Read?

The bottom of the page may be missed

Because it is not seen

Find toys that are low down and nearby?

These are easily missed

Because they are in an ‘invisible area’

Left hemianopia/inattention

How does your child…

Sit at the dining table?

Tends to adopt a slight body turn to see both knife and fork

Body turn helps compensate for hemi-inattention, more so on the left side, which tends to be more severely affected than the right

Walk along?

Walks with a slight upper body turn to the left. Collides with obstacles on the left especially when distracted or tired

Body turn minimises collisions in those with left inattention, but concentration is needed, and lack of concentration accentuates features of lack of awareness on the left

Choose to read?

Some children with acquired left hemianopia choose to read vertically upwards

Reading in this way becomes easier because the lack of vision occludes the material that has just been read and does not interfere with reading in the same way as horizontal reading

Right hemianopia/inattention

How does your child…

Draw pictures?

For some children the right side of the picture is consistently drawn poorly

Right-sided attentional dysfunction can interfere with artistic representation on the right

Choose to read?

Some children with acquired right hemianopia choose to read vertically downwards

Analogous explanation as above for left hemianopia

Impaired visual perception of movement

How well does your child see…

Moving traffic?

‘He isn’t allowed out on the road, because he doesn’t see traffic’

Slow visual, attentional or intellectual visual processing. (Independent of whether risk is appreciated)

Fast moving animals like terriers?

These animals tend to startle and frighten

They may not be seen while moving and can be described as popping out from nowhere when they stop

Rapid or fleeting facial expressions?

Affected children can miss the nuances of rapid facial expression

Impaired movement perception, delayed attention or slow intellectual processing warrant consideration

Moving balls?

Children commonly say that a kicked ball vanishes until it slows down

Suggestive of impaired perception of fast movement

Does your child prefer…

Slow or rapid film and TV?

Slow moving film and TV is preferred

Slow visual/attentional processing can preclude fast moving imagery from being seen

Pattern of ‘dorsal stream dysfunction’ – handling complex scenes

How does your child…

Find a toy in a toy box?

Strategies include: (1) empty the box and spread toys out, (2) ask an adult, (3) choose a different toy, and (4) don’t bother

Impaired visual search is limited by overlap, or the number of items that can be seen at once, which in turn impairs search and attention

Find an object on a patterned background?

Commonly missed unless background pattern and/or clutter is eliminated

The amount of pattern or clutter reaches a threshold beyond which affected children cannot identify what is being sought

Pick out an item of clothing from a pile?

The clothes are spread out to find the item that is wanted

When garments are overlapped, the lower items that are partly covered cannot be distinguished

See a distant object?

This may take time and may not be possible

In most cases, the more distant the view, the more items to be seen, and items pointed out tend not to be seen

Find someone in a group of people?

Finding a friend on the playground or a parent at the school gate is very difficult

This visual search task is difficult. Parents tend to explain that they stand in a specific location at the school gate while wearing clothing that stands out

Choose to watch the television?

Affected children get very close to the TV and move their heads to see component parts of the imagery

They have difficulty embracing the whole visual scene

Find letters on a keyboard?

This is another visual search task…

…which becomes easier as the letter locations are learned and touch typing skills mastered

Maintain eye contact?

Eye contact during conversation tends to be impaired

The dual task of viewing and interpreting the face and facial expression is unattainable by many with ‘dorsal stream dysfunction’

Find their way around in a busy place?

Disorientation is common but tends to be confined to crowded situations

Unlike the intrinsic egocentric disorientation of topographical agnosia, the disorientating factor appears to be scene complexity

Cope with team sports?

Considerable difficulty is commonly described

The combination of difficulties with visual search, and the commonly associated difficulty with movement vision, along with other factors renders team sport very difficult

Do arithmetical calculations?

School arithmetic books are often difficult to cope with

Text and image crowding, and jumbling of numbers in rows and columns, are often described as rendering arithmetic difficult

Know where you are calling from?

Inability to locate the source of a voice is commonly described

Sound source locations are likely mapped in the multimodal map of the surroundings created by posterior parietal function

React when disturbed when working?

Distraction while working can lead to angry outbursts

Schoolwork requires considerable focus. A behaviour of striking an adjacent distracting restless child is not uncommon. However, may also be caused by inability to divide attention

Behave in crowded places, e.g. shopping malls?

Crowded places are disliked and can cause discomfort, distress or anger

Crowded visual and auditory scenes can be overwhelming. However, may also be caused by inability to divide attention

Go about copying information?

Copying is difficult, slow, inaccurate and arduous

Impaired visual search is compounded because both the source and destination of copied information have to be repeatedly located

Communicate verbally?

Affected children are commonly very fluent verbal communicators

Verbal communication tends to develop well and can substitute and compensate in part for the visual difficulties

Does your child…

Always see big obvious items?

Large obvious items are intermittently missed

The nature of ‘simultanagnostic’ vision is that only a small number of items are parallel processed and perceived at once. If by chance items other than the big obvious one are being attended to, the big ‘obvious’ one may not be seen

Pattern of ‘dorsal stream dysfunction’ – guiding movement of the limbs

How does your child…

Reach out to pick something up

Reaching without looking is a commonly used approach

This commonly observed behaviour suggests that visual guidance of hand and arm movement is more easily performed in the peripheral visual field

Reach out to pick up an object from a table?

Items can be mis-reached for

Compensatory strategies used include: supplementary body or limb contact with the table, whole hand placement on the object and overreaching and gathering up

Put things down on a table?

Items may miss the table or be smashed down

Localisation of the vertical and horizontal dimensions of the table are inaccurate without tactile supplementation of visual guidance of movement

Walk over a floor boundary when looking at it?

Flat floor boundaries may be investigated by a probing foot or even hand

Steps and floor boundaries are not easily differentiated by visual means alone

Walk across a carpet with large patterns?

Some children walk around large patterns as if they are obstacles

Mapping of the surroundings for motor guidance is limited

Respond to pictorial decorations on a plate?

Some younger affected children reach out to grasp images as if they are 3D

Mapping of the image for motor guidance is deficient

Get into a bath?

Getting into a bath can be frightening

The depth of a bath can be difficult to estimate

Pattern of ‘dorsal stream dysfunction’ – impaired attention

What happens when your child…

Is walking along, and you start a conversation?

Obstacles are walked in to

Simultaneous dual processing is difficult. However, may also be caused by inability to divide attention

Is distracted while trying to do school work?

It can be very difficult to get back to what was being done

The apparently random nature of impaired parallel processing (‘simultanagnostic vision’) renders it difficult to reembrace the data set being worked on

Behave when there is an obstacle like a baby on the floor?

As a young child he/she walked straight over the baby without noticing

In some cases lower visual field impairment is accompanied by both an inability to estimate depth for motor activity and what appears to be a form of visual inattention

Pattern of ‘ventral stream dysfunction’

Does your child… a

Recognise you and photographs of you?

Varied degrees of prosopagnosia can be described

Difficulty to select and use individual facial features to recognise persons based on face perception only

Respond appropriately to facial expressions?

Facial expressions may not convey meaning

Lack of language interpretation from facial expression can be part of the clinical picture

Maintain eye contact?

Eye contact may not be made nor maintained

Eye contact tends not to be maintained by those for whom the face fails to convey information

Recognise shapes and objects?

Shapes have to be colour coded to enable recognition

Impaired shape and object discrimination and/or identification and recognition, respectively, of variable degree can be part of the picture

Correctly identify left and right shoes?

This is a common problem

Impaired analysis of shape orientation may be evident

Navigate in well-known places?

Gets lost even around the home

Disorientation due to topographical agnosia is profound

Remember where things are normally kept?

Never seems to know where things go

Impaired visual localisation of objects. Topographical agnosia also affects localisation of possessions

How does your child handle…

Strangers?

Strangers are greeted in the same way as friends and relatives

Prosopagnosia leads to strangers not being differentiated visually from those who are known

Telling the time from a clock?

Numbers may be inaccessible

Number symbols may be unintelligible

Reading?

Variants of alexia may be evident

Words may be unintelligible

Geometry?

Representational shapes and objects may be unintelligible

Difficulty envisioning shapes can render geometry difficult

The child’s emotional development

Is your child aware…

Of his/her visual condition?

Yes, no or in part

Awareness of ‘abnormal’ vision and insight into its consequences depends crucially on child’s corresponding experiences

Is your child…
   

Affected emotionally by the visual difficulties?

Yes, no or in part

As they grow up, children become aware of their differences, particularly if they are teased or bullied

Parental hopes and aspirations

What are your hopes for…

The outcome of this visit?

An aspirational description

The outcome is optimised if as many aspirations as possible are worked towards

Your child’s future?

An expression of hopes and expectations

It is important to work towards the parents’ and the child’s aspirations


While the questions are formulated for parents and caregivers, they can be easily amended to ask affected children. A spontaneously volunteered history matching this table, for which there is no alternative explanation, is highly suggestive of CVI (this list is linked to the range of supportive strategies described in Table 6.4)

aThese are more direct questions. If there is a need to do so, more indirect forms of questioning can be employed

Selecting and employing supplementary question sets as required, from a closed question inventory. Table 6.4 comprises a published set of closed leading questions (Dutton and Bax 2010) that was gradually assembled during a series of studies aimed at identifying and characterising visual behaviours in children with hydrocephalus (Houliston et al. 1999; Andersson et al. 2006) and those born prematurely (Macintyre-Béon et al. 2012, 2013), as well as seeking evidence of sets of such behaviours in a large longitudinal cohort study of children (Williams et al. 2011). In clinical practice, we recommend employing the non-leading questions described in Table 6.3 to move towards the diagnosis, proceeding to salient closed leading questions from Table 6.4 for issues that Table 6.3 questions have not clarified. Positive responses to these questions are clarified using the above technique of considering ‘time, place and person’ to gain as full an understanding as possible.


Table 6.4
Inventory of direct questions to ask of parents/carers of children with cerebral visual impairment and acuities of 6/60 or better























































































































































































































































































































































































































 
Never

Rarely

Sometimes

Often

Always

NA

Questions seeking evidence of visual field impairment or impaired visual attention on one or other side
           

Does your child…

1. Trip over toys and obstacles on the floor?
           

2. Have difficulty walking down stairs?
           

3. Trip at the edges of pavements going up?
           

4. Trip at the edges of pavements going down?
           

5. Appear to ‘get stuck’ at the top of a slide/hill?
           

6. Look down when crossing floor boundaries, e.g. where lino meets carpet?
           

7. Leave food on the near or far side of their plate?
           

If so, on which side?

Near/
 
Far
     

8. Leave food on the right or left side of their plate?
           

If so, on which side?

Right/
 
Left
     

Does your child…

9. Have difficulty finding the beginning of a line when reading?
           

10. Have difficulty finding the next word when reading?
           

11. Walk out in front of traffic? If so, which side?

Right/

Left/

Both
     

12. Bump into doorframes or partly open doors?
           

If so, which side?

Right/

Left/

Both
     

13. Miss pictures or words on one side of a page?
           

If so, which side?

Right/

Left/

Both
     

Questions seeking evidence of impaired perception of movement

Does your child…

14. Have difficulty seeing passing vehicles when they are in a moving car?
           

15. Have difficulty seeing things which are moving quickly, such as small animals?
           

16. Avoid watching fast moving TV?
           

17. Choose to watch slow moving TV?
           

18. Have difficulty catching a ball?
           

Questions seeking evidence of difficulty handling the complexity of a visual scene

Does your child…

19. Have difficulty seeing something which is pointed out in the distance?
           

20. Have difficulty finding a close friend or relative who is standing in a group?
           

21. Have difficulty finding an item in a supermarket, e.g. finding the breakfast cereal they want?
           

22. Get lost in places where there is a lot to see, e.g. a crowded shop?
           

23. Get lost in places which are well known to them?
           

24. Have difficulty locating an item of clothing in a pile of clothes?
           

25. Have difficulty selecting a chosen toy in a toy box?
           

26. Sit closer to the television than about 30 cm?
           

27. Find copying words or drawings time consuming and difficult?
           

Questions seeking evidence impairment of visually guided movement of the body and further evidence of visual field impairment

28. When walking, does your child hold onto your clothes, tugging down?
           

29. Does your child find uneven ground difficult to walk over?
           

30. Does your child bump into low furniture such as a coffee table?
           

31. Is low furniture bumped in to if it is moved?
           

32. Does your child get angry if furniture is moved?
           

33. Does your child explore floor boundaries (e.g. lino/carpet) with their foot before crossing the boundary?
           

34. Does your child find inside floor boundaries difficult to cross?
           

34a. If so…, boundaries that are new to them?
           

34b. Boundaries that are well known to them?
           

Questions seeking evidence of impairment of visually guided movement of the upper limbs

35. Does your child reach incorrectly for objects, that is, do they reach beyond or around the object?
           

36. When picking up an object, does your child grasp incorrectly, that is, do they miss or knock the object over?
           

Questions seeking evidence of impaired visual attention

37. Does your child find it difficult to keep to task for more than 5 min?
           

38. After being distracted does your child find it difficult to get back to what they were doing?
           

39. Does your child bump into things when walking and having a conversation?
           

40. Does your child miss objects which are obvious to you because they are different from their background and seem to ‘pop out’, e.g. a bright ball in the grass?

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

May 10, 2017 | Posted by in NEUROLOGY | Comments Off on Diagnostic Assessment

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access