and Gordon N. Dutton2
(1)
Department of Psychology, LMU Munich, München, Germany
(2)
Department of Visual Science, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK
7.1 Some Introductory Remarks
Brain developmental disorders and those occurring in early childhood give rise to a variety of functional impairments. This does not however necessarily mean that these impairments, and their consequences for everyday life activities, are immutable, that the developmental processes and functional capacities of the (very) young brain are persistently affected or that the capacity of adaptation to these functional consequences in terms of learning will be irreversibly reduced or lost. Thus, specific and systematic intervention measures are needed as early as possible after brain injury, or the onset of brain dysfunction.
The principal aim of all early intervention measures is to minimise the degree of disability to facilitate self-reliance and independent daily life activities, with better participation in social and school activities and programmes, leading eventually to a higher quality of life. In essence, the child with visual impairment learns from life, for life.
All our knowledge, creativity and willingness are needed to develop tailor-made and problem-oriented solutions for intervention. Assessment of children with CVI to characterise the nature of each child’s condition for the purpose of planned intervention needs close cooperation by all disciplines involved. Intervention measures first of all aim to ameliorate the degree of visual disability in everyday life and reduce and minimise social disadvantage. This can be achieved by first eliciting and quantifying the limits of each visual capacity and working within these thresholds, when catering for each of the child’s needs. Approaches aimed at improving the affected visual capacity, for example, visual acuity, as well as efficient compensation strategies to overcome each visual dysfunction, e.g. visual field defects by gaze shifts; the use of technical aids, e.g. magnifying glasses; modified parenting strategies; and environmental modification in the home, are all considered. Often a combination of functional improvement and compensation techniques is the optimal strategy. Substitution for impaired or lost visual capacity is, at least in adults, the most frequently employed measure, because recovery of visual function after acquired brain injury appears the exception rather than the rule (Zihl 2011). For children with CVI, research results concerning substitution for impaired visual capacities are still somewhat sparse compared with adults. It appears that (spontaneous) recovery and adaptation to visual dysfunction in children occurs more often, provided that other functional systems particularly involved in vision are sufficiently spared and can develop normally. Attention, learning, memory and executive function, as well as motivation to learn more about the visual world are crucial prerequisites for flexible adaptation and the use of (spared) visual capacities in an optimal way. However, as Hoyt (2003) has shown, just living in a natural environment is not sufficient to elicit improvements in vision.
In principle, the following approaches aimed at minimising cerebral visual disability are possible:
Ensure that environmental conditions and all communication and materials used are accessible to and matched to the developing needs of the child, by being supra-threshold for each of the child’s measured/estimated perceptual limitations.
Return/recovery of impaired visual capacities, spontaneously or after systematic training.
Development of efficient strategies for using impaired visual capacities (improvement of residual visual capacities).
Substitution of impaired or lost visual capacities by other functions or capacities (substitution by functional compensation).
Substitution of impaired or lost visual capacities by technical aids.
Parents and caregivers need to be closely involved in all habilitational efforts, so that they can be shown how to help their child, and encouraged to integrate and appropriately extend any successful outcomes of intervention into their child’s activities of everyday living.
7.2 Spontaneous Recovery and Spontaneous Adaptation in Children with CVI
In adults with acquired CVI, spontaneous recovery has been reported for nearly all visual capacities; however, the degree of recovery is rarely complete (Zihl 2011). Children with severe CVI may develop some useful vision (Duchowny et al. 1974; Kaye and Herskowitz 1986; Lambert et al. 1987). The temporal course of recovery from cerebral blindness follows a typical pattern. In the first phase, children are able to detect moving and flickering light stimuli. Perception of (very) bright colours, contours and simple forms follows. Some children eventually develop contrast vision and visual acuity, which allow perception and identification of forms, objects, faces, scenes, etc. However, improvement can come to an end at each stage and for each visual capacity. Remarkable visual capacities (detection and discrimination of visual stimuli, movement vision) and visually guided behaviour (fixation, tracking, reaching) have been reported in single cases with congenital absence of normal occipital cortex or early bilateral occipital injury (e.g. Dubowitz et al. 1986; Summers and MacDonald 1990; Giaschi et al. 2003) indicating that development of vision may in part also be mediated by subcortical and/or extrastriate visual structures. Bova et al. (2008) reported the case of a boy with bilateral occipital lobe infarction at the age of 2.5 years. Complete cerebral blindness lasted a few weeks, then perception of movement returned, followed by progressive recovery of visual acuity, visual field and oculomotor functions. At the age of 6 years and 8 months, the boy had developed normal acuity (10/10), but still showed incomplete bilateral upper hemianopia (the pathway of the optic radiations serving the upper visual fields runs through the temporal lobe) and difficulties with complex visual form perception (e.g. overlapping figures) and complex visual-spatial tasks (e.g. Block design, Rey’s figure). Similar patterns of recovery have been reported by Innocenti et al. (1999), Werth (2007) and Muckli et al. (2009). These reports also illustrate that spontaneous recovery from cerebral blindness may take place over a number of years. Furthermore, the degree of recovery after brain injury early in life may not always be greater than later in adulthood, i.e. plasticity in childhood may also be limited, indicating that ‘young is not always better’ because vulnerability in the developing brain may be more crucial with respect to signal transmission and neuronal connectivity (Anderson 2003; Giza and Prins 2006). On the other hand, as Cioni et al. (2011) have pointed out, some mechanisms underlying brain plasticity may no longer be available at a later stage of maturation, in particular in the visual, sensorimotor and language systems.
As with adults, children with complete cerebral blindness are not always aware of their blindness (Barnet et al. 1970). In the course of recovery from cerebral blindness, visual perceptions without external stimuli (visual hallucinations) may occur (White and Jan 1992), which can be incorrectly taken to be real, and thus may impede awareness of blindness, or correct interpretation of any vision present, or improvement of vision. Many children with complete cerebral blindness also manifest cognitive dysfunction, which may impair awareness, as well as impeding visual assessment and intervention (Barnet et al. 1970; Jan et al. 1977; see also Matsuba and Jan 2006). Interestingly, Guzzetta et al. (2010) have found stronger awareness for visual stimuli in terms of ‘conscious feelings’ in subjects with cerebral blindness after early brain injury compared with subjects who have suffered brain injury later in life. Unfortunately, the use of electrophysiological correlates of vision may yield equivocal results because children with cerebral blindness may sometimes show preserved pattern-generated VEP (Frank and Torres 1979), but for many children with CVI, there is a good correlation between VEP and preferential looking methodologies (Mackie et al. 1995) showing that results warrant interpretation in the context of the whole clinical picture.
Partial recovery of vision has been reported in children with homonymous hemianopia, impaired visual acuity and contrast sensitivity (Groenendaal and van Hof-van Duin 1990; Porro et al. 1998), with a time of recovery that may take several years. Kedar et al. (2006) have reported spontaneous recovery of vision in about 40 % of 31 children with homonymous visual field defects. Matsuba and Jan (2006) found spontaneous improvement of visual acuity over two or more years in 97 of 194 children (50 %). Seventy-five of the children (38.7 %) did not show any change in acuity, while the remainder deteriorated (18, or 9.3 %); in 4 children, acuity could not be assessed properly. Children with better visual acuity values at follow-up also showed better cognitive outcomes. Furthermore, independent mobility was associated with higher visual acuity, indicating a favourable effect of vision on motor outcome. Similar observations had been previously reported by van Hof-van Duin et al. (1998) who found that visual outcome in children with CVI could be predicted by grating acuity at the age of 12–24 months in 27 out of 39 children (69.2 %). Watson et al. (2007) reported improvement of visual acuity in 49 % of 34 children and of contrast vision in 47 % of 39 children, but no relation was found between improvement in visual acuity and the aetiology of CVI.
Surprisingly, children with early more or less total bilateral occipital lobe injury in the first year of life may show recovery/preservation of ‘low’ vision in the central portion of their visual field, which allows them to detect a visual stimulus in motion, to fixate a stimulus or to follow a stimulus with eye and head movements (Werth 2006, 2007). An even more surprising picture emerges after loss of one occipital lobe, because – in contrast to adults – children may exhibit a nearly full visual field, at least for perception of light. This preservation of visual field contralateral to occipital injury may be explained by morphological reorganisation of retinal afferent pathways to the striate cortex and thus of transformation of cortical visual field representation (Muckli et al. 2009). Alternatively, light perception may also be mediated by subcortical structures, which are known to receive afferent signals from the retina and project to the striate cortex (Werth 2008). The latter hypothesis is supported by a study in 23 children with uni- or bilateral homonymous visual field defects with spared perception of moving visual stimuli in affected visual field regions (Boyle et al. 2005). Children can also recover from visual neglect; in contrast to adulthood, it seems that this disorder tends not to become chronic in children (Ferro et al. 1984; Trauner 2003; Kleinman et al. 2010).
De Haan and Campbell (1991) presented a 15-year follow-up of a 27-year-old female patient with developmental prosopagnosia with relatively preserved basic visuo-sensory functions which were largely intact and relatively well-preserved discrimination of a face and a ‘non-face’. In contrast, recognition of familiar faces was severely impaired, and facial expression recognition was difficult, so that the patient may not have been able to learn representations of faces. Joy and Brunsdon (2002) reported spontaneous improvement of visual face discrimination and identification and recognition in a 7-year-old boy with congenital prosopagnosia, who was assessed for the first time at the age of 4 years. In contrast, impaired visual recognition of familiar faces may not show any change (see also Ariel and Sadeh 1996; Sect. 4.3.8).
A particularly impressive example of spontaneous adaptation by highly efficient compensation strategies has been reported by Lê et al. (2002). The authors assessed in detail a 30-year-old man (SB), who suffered from bilateral posterior brain injury because of meningoencephalitis at the age of 3 years. Injury affected both ventral (occipito-temporal) pathways and the right dorsal (occipito-parietal) route, as verified by MRI. From his 6th to 16th year, SB had been in an institution for visually disabled children and young adults; during the ensuing 4 years, he followed a professional training. As acquisition of visual reading ability was not possible, he learned the Braille system, which he mastered fluently. He was unable to visually recognise stimuli, but had no difficulties with tactile recognition of the same items. What was remarkable with SB was the discrepancy between his severe CVI and his visually guided activities in everyday life, including sport activities; because of this mismatch, some teachers questioned his severe visual disability. He showed more or less normal visual-spatial orientation and navigation, learned how to ride a motorcycle and played as goalkeeper in an amateur football team. There were no impairments in cognition, language or motor functions. Detailed visual assessment revealed complete left-sided homonymous hemianopia without visual neglect, the distance binocular visual acuity for forms at a distance of 5 m was estimated 0.6. He showed impaired contrast sensitivity for middle and high spatial frequencies, cerebral achromatopsia and impaired form vision. Foveal light sensitivity was normal, and stereopsis was not impaired. Visual identification and recognition were severely impaired; SB could, however, ‘guess’ real objects with the help of characteristic (individual) features and properties. A similar outcome was found for faces. Although SB could correctly discriminate faces as identical or different in pairwise presentation, he was absolutely unable to visually recognise ‘familiar’ faces and also had difficulties identifying facial expressions. Reading was impossible, because SB also showed visual agnosia for letters (pure alexia). His visual imagination of colours was completely absent, while for other visual categories (objects, animals, faces), it was rudimentary. In sharp contrast to SB’s profoundly impaired visual identification and recognition, were his capacities in topographical orientation and navigation and in visually guided grasping. Sparing of just one (i.e. the left) dorsal visual processing route and the intensive spontaneous use of spared visual capacity (and possibly residual visual capacities belonging to the ventral routes) in everyday life had ensured that visual perceptual learning became sufficient to acquire remarkable visual-spatial skills. In situations, which primarily demanded visual-spatial capacities, SB behaved like a sighted person, while in situations that demanded visual identification and recognition, he behaved like a person who had become blind. Interestingly, SB never chose to be registered as visually impaired. SB’s visuospatial capacities in some ways resemble that of case DF, the patient who inspired the dual-route processing model of Milner and Goodale (2006). However, as Schenk (2006) has convincingly argued, spared visuomotor capacities in bilateral ventral route injury can alternatively be explained by redundancy of visuomotor control (see also Schenk and McIntosh 2010). Apart from the sparing and further development of visual-spatial and possibly also visual-cognitive skills, the case of SB tells us that visual development and adaptation to everyday life visual challenges require preserved (visual) learning capacity, including visual memory, attention and executive function, and a high level of motivation for visual information (visual curiosity). Thus, an optimal combination of functional brain plasticity, adequate (visual) environment and intensive practice of visuomotor actions and activities may explain SB’s significant improvements in useful vision and thus provide an excellent example of environmental- and practice-dependent plasticity of the visual system (see Chap. 1). Visuomotor experience over several years can manifestly enhance spontaneous adaptation to early CVI.
A positive interaction between vision and motor activities has also been reported by Pavlova et al. (2007), who examined the association between visual-spatial navigation and paresis in the upper and lower limb extremities in 14 preterm children with PVL at the age of 13–16 years. Children with upper limb paresis showed higher performance in a maze task than children with lower limb paresis. The authors concluded that children with intact lower limbs, i.e. with normal walking, also showed better visual navigation. This observation is underlined by Evenson et al. (2009) who found that reduced visual acuity accords a higher risk of motor problems in preterm children but not in term children small for gestational age.
In conclusion, despite many positive examples of spontaneous recovery of visual capacities and spontaneous adaptation, many children with CVI may exhibit persistent visual impairment (see Hoyt 2003), which may cause disability well into, if not during adulthood: in addition, persistent CVI may further affect overall health, self-perception, educational attainment, job choices and social interactions (Davidson and Quinn 2011). Thus, intervention measures are required as early as possible, to reduce the degree of visual handicap; to support an optimal visual, cognitive and social development of children with CVI; and to guarantee the best options possible for later life.
In the following sections, intervention strategies in children with CVI are described and discussed. Because scientifically proven programmes of intervention in children with CVI are not available, the strategies proposed represent recommendations of intervention. Of course, intervention in children with CVI should also be based on general principles of rehabilitation. Therefore, it seems helpful to first outline some fundamental issues that are relevant for visual rehabilitation in general, and for the rehabilitation in children with CVI in particular. The proposed intervention measures and strategies are exemplified by single-case reports in Chap. 9.
7.3 Methodical Considerations in Visual Rehabilitation and Special Early Education in Children with CVI
7.3.1 Functional Visual Assessment
A crucial prerequisite for adequate intervention and special early education are assessment methods that comprise all important domains of development and fulfil the criteria of validity, reliability and objectivity (see Chap. 6) in the context of ensuring that every element of every test is easily visible to the child being assessed. These assessment methods should also possess ecological validity, i.e. should provide relevant information about the quality and quantity of individual visual disability (so-called assessment of functional vision; see, e.g. Colenbrander 2009, Boot et al. 2010; cf. Sect. 6.1). To gain such information, standardised methods for the comprehensive assessment of visual capacities should be combined with systematic observation in everyday life conditions and feedback from the child and his parents, family members and other significant persons. A further significance of functional visual assessment is the valid and reliable measurement of the effects of intervention in terms of reduction of severity of visual disability in both the clinical and the child’s own settings (Table 7.1).
Table 7.1
Summary of tasks of functional diagnostics
Assessing specific visual impairment(s) and determining all functional binocular, spectacle corrected visual capacities in the context of everyday performance (evaluating positive and negative pictures of performance) |
Assessing the degree of severity of visual impairment(s) |
Identifying skills abilities and strengths |
Deciding upon and recommending interventions, matched to the capacity of the child/family/professionals to implement them |
Assessing outcome of interventions |
Continuing the cycle |
For children with CVI, functional assessment should comprise evaluation of all visual functions and capacities (e.g. contrast vision and visual acuity for form vision), particularly in relation to the higher-level visual perceptual components that they serve. For example, the diagnosis ‘homonymous concentric restriction of the visual field’ does not communicate how wide the child’s overview might be or whether the child efficiently uses compensatory gaze shifts, for example. The same argument applies for ‘impaired spatial contrast sensitivity’. This description does not contain valid information concerning whether form vision and object and face perception are impaired, and to what degree, or not impaired at all. In essence, measures of functional vision need to be evaluated, explained and set in the context of education and everyday living.
The amount of light required for optimum visual performance needs to be considered for each child with low vision, because the ability to discriminate elements of an image starts to diminish below a critical level of background or ambient lighting, in the same way as it does for those with normal vision, but not infrequently at a higher level of lighting. On the other hand, there is a group of children with profound visual impairment due to CVI whose vision is paradoxically enhanced when light levels are reduced (Good and Hou 2006), and functional vision in this group needs to be evaluated in mesopic conditions of lighting, to determine whether performance improves.
Functional visual assessment differs from the assessment of visual function in a number of ways. Visual function assessments, such as visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, visual fields and stereopsis, are determinations of threshold levels of vision. In particular, visual acuity is measured to determine the resolution of the visual system with each eye in turn, tested at maximum contrast ideally under specified bright lighting conditions in defined standard conditions. In contrast, assessment of functional vision is carried out under ambient lighting conditions, with both eyes open with optimal spectacle correction if required, with the aim of determining what can be reliably, consistently and comfortably seen and appreciated in daily life, ideally both when wide awake in the morning and when tired later in the day (recognising the increased fatiguability of the visual system in many children with CVI). The functional visual acuity, for example, tends to be two- to threefold larger than the measured visual acuity, which for everyone, sighted and visually impaired alike, denotes the lower limit of visual function. (Fully sighted people do not choose to read text at their level of binocular visual acuity; they prefer text which is two- to threefold larger, reflecting their functional visual acuity; Lueck 2004.) It is clearly very important that threshold acuity measures are not communicated as those to be used by families and professionals working with children with CVI, as all visual information presented needs to be well within the perceptual limitations of the child. Visual acuity specifies the minimum perceptible line thickness and minimum perceptible gap thickness between lines found in text or imagery, whereas the functional visual acuity specifies the measure that ensures that all elements are visible during everyday viewing in ambient lighting.
Similarly, functional contrast sensitivity assessment is carried out to ensure that all adjacent greyscale elements of all images presented are perceptible, as contrast sensitivity can be diminished in children with CVI (Good et al. 2012). Greyscale black-and-white images as seen in black-and-white photography are now not commonly shown to children. Colour contrast also needs to be considered. For example, light blue is blue to which white has been added, and navy blue is blue to which black has been added. The ability to differentiate these two colours is thus an evaluation of contrast sensitivity in the context of a blue background. This conceptual model applies to all colours and needs to be born in mind by all those investigating, looking after and teaching children with low vision.
Colour vision in its own right is also evaluated from a functional standpoint, by evaluating which adjacent colours, at which degrees of saturation can be differentiated, and by determining which coloured objects are identified or missed against which backgrounds.
The functional visual field embraces a number of concepts. Apart from eye movement detection methods, which show considerable promise in evaluating attentional visual fields in children (Murray et al. 2009), the visual field is commonly assessed with the head and eyes looking in a single direction. On the other hand, functional visual field evaluation assesses whether there is lack of visual function in any area of the visual field, during everyday living. It takes into account the visual field per se, visual attention throughout the visual field, any reflex vision that brings about saccadic eye movement to peripheral targets and any divergent strabismus compensating for hemianopia, as well as detrimental features such as tonic eye movements that limit visual access in the direction opposite to the deviation.
The peripheral lower visual field is needed for walking and running. Thus, the functional lower visual field is evaluated with both eyes open while looking straight ahead. The subject is given support, if needed, while elevating a straight leg until the foot is seen, for each leg in turn. Functional peripheral lower visual field impairment is evident if the foot has to be elevated to a degree that would encompass more than two paces ahead (more than 20°). (Standard visual field testing employs targets that do not evaluate this peripheral area of the visual field, so that peripheral lower visual field impairment interfering with walking may not be identified using standard measures.)
Evidence of impaired parallel visual processing and its degree are elicited in children for whom prior history taking reveals impaired visual search. The number of items that can be simultaneously perceived is elicited, and the degrees to which background pattern and surrounding clutter interfere with this process during everyday living are evaluated.
The accuracy of visual guidance of movement of the arms and hands, and the legs and feet, in the context of everyday living, is sought by both history taking and assessment.
Key issues to consider include:
1.
Comprehensive evaluation of the day-to-day consequences of dysfunction of the central visual system and its effects on behaviour and individual experiences of the child.
2.
Functional visual assessment to cover the quality (type) and quantity (degree of severity) of the child’s visual impairments in a valid and reliable way, in relation to the child’s developmental age.
3.
Evaluation and communication of the spared visual capacities (providing both ‘positive’ and ‘negative’ pictures of development and performance).
4.
In children with CVI, developmental stages of cognition, motor functions and activities, as well as language, motivation, emotion and social behaviour, are also assessed, and the results of visual assessment are interpreted in this context. This form of interpretation is important, because it potentially allows differentiation between primary and secondary visual impairments (e.g. due to attentional dysfunction) and informs the need for intervention measures and specialist early education (impairments in these functional domains should of course be treated separately and specifically, when required).
5.
The effects of intervention and special early education should be strictly monitored; this helps avoid inappropriate or suboptimal application of intervention measures and can be used to change treatment or education programmes as required. All experts in this field, who are responsible for the child’s further development, are primarily responsible for proper assessment and intervention, ideally based on scientifically proven or at least face-value evidence.
6.
Although intervention and the effects of special early education are assessed using appropriate measures, the main outcome measure is useful vision. Of course, one is on the right track, if after intervention the child shows improved visual acuity, but the crucial test of efficacy is ecological validity, i.e. whether the child benefits from improved acuity in visual localisation, spatial orientation and navigation, form vision, object and face perception and visual recognition.
7.
The results of visual assessment, the conclusions reached and recommended actions to be taken need to be communicated to all interested parties in easily understood language. Specific advice on care and parenting includes information about how not to criticise behaviours due to CVI but instead to support the child, along with description of specific targeted strategies that the child, caregivers and family can all implement on a day-to-day basis (Sect. 7.4).
7.3.2 Requirements of Intervention Measures
Successful adaptation to the challenge of brain dysfunction particularly during development depends on two main prerequisites: enriched environments and systematic experience (van Praag et al. 2000; Nithianantharajah and Hannan 2006; Kolb et al. 2011; Eckert and Abraham 2013). In addition, the availability of learning capacity, including cognitive (perception, attention, memory, executive function) and motor functions involved in learning, also appears crucial, because in the absence of the critical minimum of learning capacity, interactions between enriched environment and systematic experience cannot take place, i.e. environment- and experience-dependent plasticity cannot operate. In children with CVI, the enriched environment may be understood as stimulus conditions, which on the one hand support the child’s endeavours and on the other make sufficient demands on a child’s visual and cognitive equipment at a given time of individual development, but do not over expend the child’s capacities. Thus, adequate visual sensory enrichment is optimally adapted to the child’s visual-cognitive capacities, which also includes controlled intervention conditions. On the other hand, experience-dependent plasticity implies that systematic practice is required for visual perceptual and visual-cognitive learning to lead to improvement of vision in the context of action and thus behaviour. Systematic practice should, therefore, consist of a fair balance between given (and assumed) visual, cognitive and motor capacity of a child and tailor-made task demands that are essential for successful intervention measures. Both enriched environments and systematic practice with the use of such environments in the behavioural context are known to modulate neuronal efficacy and thus functional brain capacity (van Praag et al. 2000; Berlucchi 2011; Eckert and Abraham 2013).
For children with CVI, the concept of enrichment includes the need for all training to be administered in an environment free from clutter and noise and in a creative way by skilled individuals, in a form of motivational play that is interesting, challenging, varied, rewarding and fun. Children with CVI have to work hard to ‘see’ and are easily fatigued, which can manifest as lack of engagement and sometimes distress. This needs to be recognised and positively catered for. Children need to be happy and wide awake to actively participate and learn.
As already mentioned in the introduction to this chapter, adequate application of interventional measures in children with CVI requires professional diagnostic assessment with mastery of the methods of intervention and early special education, which are tailor-made and individually adapted to the complexity of the child’s individual challenges.
Intervention procedures may be non-specific or specific and systematic or nonsystematic.
Non-specific intervention aims at general (i.e. non-specific) activation of visual functions. They are indicated whenever a child shows little spontaneous activity, because of global or specific (visual curiosity) motivation difficulties, or insufficient attention (alertness, sustained attention, mental processing, concentration). Non-specific methods are, however, unsuitable for specific treatment of visual impairments where their application may even prove inappropriate.
Specific intervention allows tailor-made treatment of visual impairment, i.e. intervention is adapted to the individual type and severity of defined visual impairment. Thereby it is ensured that improvement of the visual impairment in question is the focus of intervention and of main therapeutic activities. However, it should be recognised that the higher the specificity of the method of intervention, the higher the demands for the child. If the requisite (cognition, motivation, etc.) requirements are not available, the degree of specificity can be diminished, or, in the extreme case, the child can initially be afforded adequate non-specific intervention, followed later by specific intervention.
Nonsystematic intervention comprises inconsistent performance in relation to stimulus and/or task conditions, instruction, duration (including breaks) and time of intervention, type of mediation of learning strategies and time and form of feedback. This ‘form’ of intervention has limited application and should therefore be avoided, because both the child and the therapist cannot develop a clear structured approach to intervention. Furthermore, no valid framework for the assessment of the outcome of such intervention can be established.
Systematic intervention, in contrast, is based on standardised rules concerning stimulus and task conditions and instructions; defined duration of intervention phases, including time points, length of breaks and time of intervention (i.e. excluding unfavourable times of the day); and regular and unequivocal feedback, which is focused on specific aspects of intervention that can be easily grasped, mastered and willingly implemented by the child. Systematic conditions of intervention also imply that the child can become sufficiently familiar with the intervention procedure and thus gain certainty, which supports self-confidence and enhances motivation and attention and guarantees compliance. Of course, systematic intervention should not be understood and applied in too stringent a form, for example, concerning time structure. If, for example, a chosen treatment poses excessive demands on a child, then the structure is simplified; this clearly poses more demands on the teacher or therapist, but it also renders intervention more interesting and effective.
Developing a sensible, systematic and comprehensive plan of intervention in a ‘holistic’ context requires a stepwise procedure. The definition and monitoring of intermediate goals of intervention and the flexible adaptation of the process of intervention to its principal aims but also to the personal needs of the child in the context of individual demands in everyday life are a complex challenge. This challenge is best guided by evidence-based knowledge, personal experience and (professional) empathy. The plan of intervention is based on the negative and positive pictures of visual capacity, on existing proven procedures and means of intervention and on ecological validity. Close cooperation between all professional disciplines engaged in the intervention for children with CVI is, therefore, essential for the success of the intervention and early special education and, of course, for the child and his or her family. Regular exchange of information and individual experiences facilitates a flexible response to individual adaptation of intervention measures and also guarantees early identification and modification of unsuccessful training procedures or training conditions. Furthermore, visual stimulus materials used for practice should always also include affective components (e.g. colours) or an affectively potent context that uplifts mood, because task-irrelevant affective information is known to negatively influence visual cortical activity (Damaraju et al. 2009).
7.3.3 Perceptual Learning and the Requisite Cognitive Capacities
Improvement of vision in children with CVI is mainly based on perceptual learning, ensuring that materials used fall within each child’s level of functional vision. The most basic form of visual perceptual learning is same-different discrimination, in which the number of stimulus features increases. More complex forms require acquisition of visual constancy and categorisation and ultimately identification and recognition (Goldstone 1998). Perceptual learning is based on brain plasticity (Fiorentini and Berardi 1997; Gilbert et al. 2009) and constitutes a fundamental type of learning in the pre-semantic period of child development, which however involves already attained complex cognitive capacities, e.g. problem solving (Coldren and Colombo 1994).
Perceptual learning has been shown to be efficient in children (and adults) with perceptual disorders with and without associated cognitive dysfunction (Greenfield 1985; Serna et al. 1997; Huurneman et al. 2013). An important factor in perceptual learning is the form of feedback. It has been shown that disabled children in particular learn to discriminate stimuli faster and with greater reliability, when errors are prevented straight away (so-called errorless learning; Sidman and Stoddard 1967). For example, in a typical visual discrimination paradigm, a child is shown a pair of stimuli of the same dimension, brightness, form or colour. Initial stimuli clearly differ, but differences are decreased stepwise, so that the child learns to detect increasingly fine differences in the respective stimulus dimensions, and the child’s sensitivity for the stimulus dimensions improves. Feedback concerning correct and incorrect discrimination, respectively, is given immediately after each response; this procedure enhances sensitivity and prevents unfavourable discrimination criteria being learned. According to the level of discrimination performance and ability, feedback can be progressively diminished. Children who understand and use verbal instruction can respond verbally; if this is not the case, fixation, pointing or grasping at the correct stimulus, possibly associated with head shaking (‘yes’-‘no’), can be used as alternative behavioural responses. Video-based recordings of the child’s responses may be helpful for later analysis of responses in the various discrimination conditions; in addition, the child benefits from the therapist’s overt interest in his responses and his attention to the stimuli during practice (joint attention).
As mentioned earlier, cognition is a crucial prerequisite for perceptual learning. In the following section, cognitive capacities required for perceptual learning in children with CVI are briefly summarised and commented upon. A wide range of issues need to be considered with respect to age and level of visual, intellectual and motor function.
7.3.3.1 Attention
The following attentional capacities need to be considered:
Sufficient alertness;
Sufficient sustained attention with respect to intensity and duration;
Sufficient concentration (focusing attention to a particular stimulus and reduction/prevention of distractibility);
Capacity for dividing attention (paying of attention to two or more stimuli in the visual and/or other modalities in parallel).
In this context, distractions including discomfort, extraneous sound and visual stimuli and social distraction are identified and minimised as appropriate.
The interplay between attention and complexity of the visual task should be considered when planning visual perceptual practice; the more complex the task, the more attentional resources are required to perform it. Regular changes in the visual material and in task conditions (e.g. detection vs. discrimination) can enhance maintenance of attention (Fantz 1964). During early special education, stimuli with high attentional value, e.g. glittering stimuli and moving simple (unicoloured balls) and complex stimuli (travelling little cars, self-moving dolls, mobiles, etc.), presented against a contrasting plain background have been found very useful as a means of catching and maintaining attention in children with low vision (e.g. Hyvarinen 2000).
7.3.3.2 Learning and Memory
Optimal conditions for learning through intervention can be achieved by:
Favourable conditions for attention/concentration and motivation;
A good relationship with the therapist;
Establishing a positive mood (frame of mind) for learning (interest in visual material and joy with practice tasks);
Avoiding taxing and insufficient demands (mental over or under load);
Regular affirmatory social signals to the child rewarding the performance despite an evident low level of performance.
For shaping practice conditions, the following considerations can prove helpful:
Transparent organisation of the amount of information, with clearly limited packages of information belonging together;
Pre-structuring of information: to (a) render tasks unambiguous to bring about an expected ‘performance’, (b) bring about global before detailed (local) processing and (c) accord recognisable significance to the stimuli;
Transfer to other task/environmental conditions: regular repetition of tasks initially under equal and then later under changing context conditions;
Confirming/rewarding: affirming, establishing and reinforcing the processes of self-monitoring of learning strategies (by giving unequivocal positive feedback; see above);
Ecological validity: establishing the concrete relevance of the tasks to everyday life situations (so that the child understands what the purpose of learning is).
7.3.3.3 Executive Functions
When considering the cognitive and developmental level and the importance of integration of the perceptual learning into broader concepts of knowledge combined with a coherent framework of guidance and training in behaviour, the following issues can prove helpful:
Visual perceptual tasks should be simple and concrete as far as possible;
The complexity of tasks should be increased in steps, taking the child’s mental processing into account, i.e. building associations between stimuli (colours, forms, objects), discovering logical connections between stimuli, predicting what may happen when …, etc.;
Support of realistic self-assessment of visual capacities and visual performance as a solid basis for developing self-control.
7.3.3.4 Visuomotor Skills
Children with posterior parietal damage or dysfunction ranging in severity from dorsal stream dysfunction to Balint syndrome can manifest a range of disorders of visual guidance of movement (optic ataxia) that may compound cerebral palsy or may occur in the context of otherwise normal motor function. Accurate visual guidance of movement is predicated upon intact visual function, visual attention and the dorsal stream-mediated capacity to create an accurate internal emulation of the surroundings, to inform the motor cortex to bring about limb or body movement with accurate visual guidance (Milner and Goodale 2006).
7.3.4 Children with CVI Without Additional Cognitive Impairments
In children with CVI, but spared cognition, intervention can be primarily focused on improvement of vision, although the developmental cognitive stage should always be taken into account. Intervention may be arranged such that visual stimulus complexity and task demands are increased stepwise with regard to the defined (intermediate) goals of systematic practice. Table 7.2 summarises various aspects of both components.
Table 7.2
Classification of visual stimulus dimensions, of stimulus complexity and of visual perceptual performance
Visual stimuli can be classified according to individual or combined features and context: |
Stimulus features |
Brightness |
Size |
Colour |
Form |
Combination of features |
Number, type of combination; level of complexity |
Context conditions |
e.g. same or different context; figure-ground relations |
Complexity of a task can be qualified in terms of: |
Detecting stimuli (type and number), |
Comparing stimuli (same-different; new-familiar; smaller-larger, etc.), |
Classifying (based on stimulus features; increase in number of features), |
Identifying (based on stimulus features; increase in number of features), |
Recognising (based on characteristic stimulus features; number of features; short- vs. long-term storing), |
Manipulating (acting with objects based on particular object features), |
Naming of visual stimuli (forms, colours, objects, faces), |
Gaining knowledge (about context, functional aspects, ‘history’ of an individual object or person, personal experiences with a particular object/person, factual knowledge; visual semantic and episodic memory) |
Important
Successive levels of task difficulty should not be implemented before the child has reached the level of visual performance required for each level.
The requisite cognitive and (fine) motor functions need to be considered for each task and must fall within each of the thresholds and capabilities of the child.
7.3.5 Children with CVI with Additional Cognitive Impairments
Some recommendations are given below concerning intervention methods to use for children with CVI with additional impairments of attention and learning/memory.
7.3.5.1 Insufficient Attentional Capacities
1.
Establishing an adequate span of attention (sustained attention for at least 5–10 min) by using visual material and task conditions that do not pose particular difficulties for the child. Distracting stimuli including background pattern and clutter should be avoided. Other modalities (audition, touch) may be included to supplement but not substitute for vision.
2.
Increase the span of attention to about 10–15 min by using complementary visual material and task conditions. Visual material should be diversified to enhance and maintain concentration and interest.
3.
Increase in (visual) attentional task demands concerning material and task condition.
4.
Practice with directing attention at particular visual stimuli or stimulus (object) features while ‘neglecting’ unessential features.
5.
Extension of orientation of attention from one visual stimulus (object) to several stimuli (or objects) (aimed at developing the capacity to shift and divide attention and to carry out parallel processing).
6.
Combinations of (1)–(5) to further increase the span of attention (30 min) and the capacity to handle diversity.
7.
Practice of sustained attention/concentration and mental load under ‘natural’ conditions.
Important
At the beginning of any intervention, the stimulus and task conditions should avoid all forms of distraction.
Intervention procedures should be planned such that the child possesses the requisite vision and attention/capacity to perform the task.
Successive levels of task difficulty should not be implemented until the child has gained the requisite attentional performance.
7.3.5.2 Insufficient Learning and Memory Capacities
The capacity to learn is the principal resource underpinning intervention, involving the acquisition of visual skills and the differentiation of visual perceptual capacities. Learning is needed when a very young child intends to fixate, observe and inspect a (complex) visual stimulus or to discriminate visual stimuli on the basis of different features. The outcome of such learning is stored in visual memory. Thus, even acquisition of ‘lower’ visual perceptual capacities requires learning and memory. Children with CVI commonly need to learn how to see, by developing activities and strategies to accurately process visual stimuli, or they may need to relearn such activities and strategies. Consideration of some main principles of learning may thus be useful in the context of intervention.
Development of learning strategies and learning habits is the crucial basis for:
The acquisition and successful use of (individual) visual experiences and factual visual knowledge;
The selection of executive activities (stored in episodic and semantic memory) and actions (stored in procedural memory) for the visual guidance of behaviour in diverse life situations (skills), including previous (planning) and later control (feedback) of the outcome of intentional behaviour;
The development of long-term visual experiences and resulting routines and habits, which can be used as (successful) routines in the future;
The development of optimal visual guidance of movement of both upper and lower limbs.
The development of visual routines and habits is a life-long process in healthy individuals also and includes flexible adaptation whenever needed, to cope successfully with the visual world and its demands. Table 7.3 summarises components of intervention in children with CVI and impaired learning/memory.
Table 7.3
Some components of intervention in children with CVI and impaired learning/memory
1. Practising single and simple visually guided activities: bringing about sufficient accuracy and duration of fixation without additional visual-perceptual demands |
2. Practising of successive visually guided activities: e.g. fixation shifts between two or more stimuli without additional visual-perceptual demands, and guidance of limb movements with methods of tactile supplementation of the visual guidance of movement |
3. Practising (learning) of: |
Detection |
Discrimination |
Identification |
Recognition |
Association with personal experience |
Association with (semantic) knowledge (including naming) |
Important
Action supports learning – perceptual learning entails action (gaze; pointing and grasping; drawing and constructing).
Learning should always occur under conditions that are as error-free as possible. Immediate supportive feedback about the child’s correct/incorrect approaches is important and facilitates faster and safer learning.
Feedback should be simple, correct and unequivocal. Otherwise, the child with CVI may be put in a condition of excessive demand or may be overloaded with too much unclear information, which is useless and cannot (and should not!) be remembered.
The child finds it difficult to understand why his or her responses are correct or incorrect.
7.4 Direct Interventions for the Visual Impairments of CVI
Direct intervention for specific visual impairments in children with CVI aims to ameliorate impaired visual capacities and compensate for irreversible visual impairments. For the individual child, it depends whether there is sufficient visual capacity in evidence, to work towards direct improvement, or whether compensation strategies will be sufficient. There is limited evidence for efficacy for direct intervention to improve vision in children with CVI (Williams et al. 2014), but lack of evidence for efficacy does not constitute evidence of lack of efficacy, and further systematic investigation in this area is required.
A good example is the work of Malkowicz et al. (2006) who showed that children with CVI can significantly benefit from systematic practice with discrimination and identification of visual stimuli.
The functional organisation of visual perception in the brain is based on parallel and serial processing and coding, organised within a form of hierarchy, in the sense that more complex (‘higher’) visual capacities depend, at least in part, on less complex, i.e. ‘lower’ visual capacities (see Chap. 2). For specific interventions, a sequence of steps emerges from the following functional organisation of the visual brain:
Visual exploration and visual search processes
Visual localisation, essential for accurate fixation and grasping
Visual contrast sensitivity and visual acuity
Visual space perception, in particular visual-spatial orientation and navigation
Visual guidance of movement of the limbs and body
Discrimination and classification of colours and forms
Discrimination, classification and identification/recognition of complex stimuli, e.g. figures, objects, faces
Visual memory (visual experience)
7.4.1 Visual Field and Field of Attention, Visual Exploration and Visual Search
The close association between detection of stimuli in the visual field and attention forms a useful basis for treating children with impaired detection in parts of the visual field.
7.4.1.1 Visual Field
It is unclear whether and to what extent vision can be restored by systematic training in adults with homonymous visual field loss (see Zihl 2011). A similar picture emerges for children with homonymous visual field defects; in single cases, recovery of vision has been reported after systematic practice with detection of slowly moving light targets along the visual field border requiring fixation and pursuit of the target, while no visual field changes were observed in the group without such practice (Werth and Moehrenschlager 1999; Werth and Seelos 2005). Based on fMRI outcome, the authors interpreted recovery of visual field as training-enhanced activity in spared striate and extrastriate visual cortices. In adults, the method of visual field restitution is still questioned, because only few patients have been reported to benefit from restitution training and the degree of visual field enlargement is usually fairly small. Moreover, restitution training is very demanding and time consuming, and evidence of (ecological) validity of this method has yet to be sufficiently proven (see Zihl 2011). In children, visual field restitution in single cases after systematic practice is a similarly interesting phenomenon, but empirical evidence with respect to the efficacy and efficiency of this treatment measure is as limited as it is for adults.
In visual field regions with depressed visual function (so-called cerebral amblyopia), visual performance (detection and localisation) may be improved by combined focusing of attention to the particular visual region to facilitate detection and localisation of the stimulus used. As a result, children may learn to spontaneously direct attention to affected visual field regions, leading to benefits in everyday life due to being more reliably able to detect both obstacles, and items that are being sought (Mezey et al. 1998). For this type of training, large moving uncoloured stimuli (black on white background or white on black background) and high-contrast coloured stimuli are used, which are initially presented in the centre (straight-ahead direction of gaze) and then at increasingly greater distances from the centre towards the periphery of the visual field. This stimulus condition facilitates stimulus detection and guides attention to where stimuli will appear. The session starts by seeking responses from the centre of the stimulus array, which is indicated by a big red light spot. Positive responses to target appearance comprise searching and orienting gaze shifts (eyes and head, later eyes only) or, if possible, additional verbal responses (yes-no). The positioning of stimulus appearance initially follows a systematic approach, but is later varied in a random way. This procedure establishes an expectation of stimulus appearance at variable positions in space and enhances the distribution of the child’s anticipatory attention throughout the visual field. When the child’s detection rate is at the level defined in the plan of treatment for a particular stimulus condition (e.g. 75–90 % correct responses), smaller stimuli and stimuli with lower contrast can be used to improve visual sensitivity. Initially, the stimulus presentation time should be as long as needed for reliable detection, but it is then successively reduced to speed up the process of visual search. A diminishing latency and an increasing accuracy of response indicate improvement in performance. Registration of eye movements and/or of pointing and grasping movements to targets by means of video- or infrared-recording techniques allows a more detailed analysis of speed and accuracy of detection and the gaze shifts involved. This approach can also be used for follow-up and longitudinal assessments (see also Sect. 6.2).
7.4.1.2 Field of Attention, Visual Exploration and Visual Search
The consistent association between directed visual attention, detection of a visual stimulus and subsequent localisation by gaze shifts can be used for the development or optimisation of oculomotor scanning strategies, which the child can, for example, also use to compensate for his or her visual field loss. The procedure is the same as described in the last section (‘visual field’). The principal difference lies in the sequence of stimulus presentation. The stimulus is first presented in the spared visual field, then along the visual field border and finally outside the visual field. By this procedure, the field of visual search and thus of attention are systematically enlarged towards the periphery. After the child has developed systematic and reliable searching movements, stimuli are presented at random positions in both hemifields within and outside the spared visual field. After this phase, scenes (pictures, photographs, defined visual search arrays, possibly presented using an LCD screen), with a big red light spot in the centre (or in the LCD frame appearing before scene presentation) as standard reference position before scanning, are very useful to ensure the same starting point. The scene should extend at least 30° to the left and right and 15–20° upwards and downwards (total diameter, 60 × 30–40°). Set size (number of stimuli) can be increased stepwise, so that the child learns to search for more than one stimulus. At the final level of difficulty, a combination of targets and distractors may be used; however, distractors should differ clearly from targets (e.g. in size, form, colour), and the child should know the target(s), before the search is started. The use of the left hand for reaching for stimuli in left hemispace and externally alerting stimulation by sounds may be helpful in reducing left-sided visual neglect (Dobler et al. 2003). As the right posterior parietal lobe maps the surroundings with respect to the body, left-sided neglect relates to the body (Ting et al. 2011) and tends not compensated for by movement of the head and eyes. Instead, rotation of the body to the left, allows the midline of the body schema to point to the left side widening the field of visual attention to include more of the left side. Affected children commonly sit at table in this way, and when they do, food is no longer left on the left side of the plate. A slight rotation of the torso to the left when walking can also be seen as a probable adaptive response in some affected children, who commonly have an additional left hemiplegia (see Fig. 7.1).


Fig. 7.1
Diagram showing left hemianopia due to right occipital pathology (a, b and c) and left hemi-inattention due to right focal posterior parietal pathology (d–f). For left hemianopia: (a) Looking straight ahead, the white disc is not seen. (b) Turning the head to the left, the white disc is revealed, but (c) by turning the body it is not. For left inattention: (d) Looking straight ahead, the white disc is not seen. (e) Turning the head to the left does not reveal the white disc, but (f) by turning the body it is revealed
7.4.2 Contrast Vision and Visual Acuity
For improving spatial contrast sensitivity and visual acuity, several procedures, which have been developed for treatment of childhood amblyopia, are available. It is, however, still unclear whether these procedures are also effective in children with CVI. It seems essential that, whatever procedure is used, active perceptual learning is involved, because ‘passive’ procedures (e.g. covering the eye with the better acuity and contrast sensitivity) alone are less efficient, at least for distant vision (Levi and Li 2009; Polat et al. 2009). Huurneman et al. (2013) used a perceptual learning paradigm (form and letter search and discrimination using PL) to improve near visual acuity in 45 children with visual impairment. Training included 12 sessions, 30 min each (two sessions weekly in 6 weeks). After practice, children showed significant improvements in near visual acuity. Binocular paradigms, where the child is actively engaged in a ‘game’ in which each eye is independently presented with separate elements of the task with the clarity of image presented to the better eye degraded to match the vision of the better eye, are showing promise (Knox et al. 2012).
7.4.3 Visual Space Perception and Spatial Orientation
7.4.3.1 Visual Localisation
Visual localisation of stimuli can be practised by employing tasks in which this visual capacity plays the most prominent role, e.g. fixation or, pointing to and grasping at simple visual stimuli or objects. It is important that any compounding effects of oculomotor deficiency (e.g. eye muscle paresis) (Weir et al. 2000) or motor dysfunction of both the upper extremities are recognised and taken into account. In the first phase of intervention, a visual stimulus (e.g. a small white or black ball with sufficient contrast attached to a thin rod) is presented in front of the child. The size of the stimulus is, of course, chosen to fall within the child’s functional visual acuity. The child is asked to look at (fixate) the stimulus and to reach for and grasp it. Children with biparietal pathology commonly manifest impaired visual guidance of movement (optic ataxia) of the upper limbs that may occur in isolation or may be integral with the features of cerebral palsy. In this context, the opportunity needs to be given to supplement visual guidance of movement with tactile guidance. Potential approaches include:
Encouraging the child to move a hand along a table to reach out for a target.Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel
Full access? Get Clinical Tree
