, Jeffrey R. Strawn2 and Ernest V. Pedapati3
(1)
Division of Psychiatry and Child Psychiatry, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH, USA
(2)
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA
(3)
Division of Psychiatry and Child Psychiatry Division of Child Neurology, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, OH, USA
If I had to give a young writer some advice I would say to write about something that has happened to him; it’s always easy to tell whether a writer is writing about something that has happened to him or something he has read or been told.
— Gabriel García Márquez
This book will provide the novice or experienced child and adolescent psychiatrist or psychotherapist an objective view of how the advances in attachment theory, systems theory, developmental psychology, and neuroscience have contributed to the contemporary practice of psychotherapy in children and adolescents. Consequently, with these advances, child and adolescent psychodynamic psychotherapy has transitioned from a traditional one-person model to a contemporary two-person relational psychotherapy model to an understanding of the psychological development of children and adolescents.
We define traditional one-person psychology as that which is based on traditional psychoanalytic concepts and the form of technique that emphasizes the role of the psychotherapist as an objective observer (one-person) of the patient’s ego defenses (the symptoms) and the discoverer of the truth regarding the patient’s intrapsychic conflicts and object relations (the patient’s inner life). Further, a goal of the traditional one-person model is to make the repressed and unconscious conflicts conscious through the use of interpretations by the psychotherapist.
We define two-person relational psychology as a model based on the integration of innate genetic, biological, psychosocial, and cultural factors. The psychotherapeutic intervention occurs at an implicit level, with the goal of a new emotional experience to create more adaptive patterns of interaction with others. To this, the psychotherapist and patient are mutually engaged in each other’s subjectivities (two-person) and states of mind. The two-person relational psychotherapist facilitates the psychotherapeutic process through enactments and self-disclosures that frequently occur unknowingly by the psychotherapist, although, at times, can be carefully timed, to move along the psychotherapeutic relationship. As Wachtel (2010) states, “two-person psychology takes the relationships seriously.” The choice of the term two-person relational psychology is designed to convey to the reader that in any here-and-now interaction, there are two present and active people mutually influencing each other’s subjectivities in the intersubjective field.
Although the interaction may be of two people (e.g., patient and psychotherapist), it is important to note that, in vivo, these processes are actively influenced and directed by previous relational experiences. In contrast to a traditional one-person model, the in vivo experiences in a two-person relational model require that the psychotherapist be “real” and genuine to allow the patient to implicitly know about his or her state of mind. That is, each person brings to the interaction a multitude of relational experiences stored in nondeclarative memory systems that influence the encounter, which are not limited to the relationship to their parents or primary caregivers. Nondeclarative memory refers to a dynamic nonconflicted unconscious also referred to by the Boston Change Process Study Group as “nonconscious” (Chap. 5). The nonconscious processes begin during infancy and help the infant implicitly learn how to perform certain tasks without the use of conscious thought. Everyday examples include: walking, talking, smiling, laughing, playing, and riding a bike. The nondeclarative memory is also known as implicit or procedural memory. Infantile memories from early childhood are not accessible to verbal or symbolic recall, but rather are encoded within the nondeclarative memory system machinery that is unique to each child and is driven by their innate cognitive abilities (or limitations). A person’s representational models are fine-tuned in perpetuity as they make sense of the meaning, affect, and intentions of others within the intersubjective field. Nondeclarative memory system is distinct from Freud’s “conflicted unconscious.” Freud’s conflicted unconscious is a central tenet to all one-person psychologies (i.e., drive, ego, object relations, and self) and is a concept that adheres to an innate psychic determinism, where nothing occurs at random; all mental events are caused by the interplay of the unconscious forces between the id, ego, and superego that preceded the event (see Chaps. 2 and 6) and are consciously retrievable through free associations and dreams.
In this book, we will review the work of major psychodynamic psychoanalysts and psychotherapists over the last century (Table 1.1); our goal is to both applaud and critique their unique contributions. In addition, we will demonstrate the seductive nature of the pursuit of understanding human behavior by each theoretical school.

Table 1.1
Evolution of traditional one-person and two-person relational psychologies

For some, the transformation of the scientific advances in early childhood development into clinical applications has been viewed with a great deal of skepticism. It is reminiscent of the manner in which the works of John Bowlby, Donald Winnicott, and Donald Fairbairn were initially viewed, with skepticism, as they gave importance to the actual loving relationship between the infant and the mother while distancing themselves from Freud’s drive theory and Klein’s object relations theory.
To date, the schism between the schools of traditional one-person psychology and two-person relational psychology remains, and we view this as problematic for the young clinician. Under the broad umbrella of psychodynamic psychotherapy for children and adolescents, there are those that hold to the importance of the Oedipus complex and the internal life of the child in the form of object relations when helping the child. In essence, the traditional one-person psychology model can be viewed as a long journey with the psychotherapist leading the patient to the excavation site considered by the psychotherapist to hold the valuable artifacts. Over time, using specialized tools, a careful reconstruction of the original site is accomplished by removing the suspected calamity, thereby ultimately resulting in a return to happiness. For many of us who are physicians or academics, this approach is not only personally appealing but consistent with our years of training. Yet, we would offer that such an approach to psychodynamic psychotherapy is somewhat lonely and distant for the patient, as the psychotherapist is an objective observer and thus considered a “one-person” model.
Under the same umbrella of psychodynamic psychotherapy for children and adolescents, are those— authors included—that have incorporated the advances in developmental scientific research in the form of meaning-making processes, emotional availability, social reciprocity, relational schemas, implicit working memory, and right brain regulatory functions into their theoretical understanding. These functions underscore the complexities in understanding that what may appear as a developmental or behavioral disturbance in the present may ultimately represent patterns imbedded in disturbances of the original caregiver–infant relationships (Sameroff and Emde 1989). We posit that the difference from a “one-person” model is that in the “two-person” model, the original caregiver–infant relationships are stored in a dynamic nonconscious at an implicit preverbal level and are not retrievable through autobiographic recall. As the title of this book suggests, the authors are very much immersed in a contemporary two-person relational model of psychodynamic psychotherapy for children and adolescents that integrates concepts of intersubjectivity —“the capacity to share, know, understand, empathize with, feel, participate in, resonate with, enter into the lived subjective experience from another” (Stern 2005)—and neuroscience (see Chap. 5). Emde (2009) further states, “Much that is important to us as human beings is shared implicitly. We now have a neurobiology of intersubjectivity that is rapidly accumulating new knowledge; it involves basic nonconscious functioning and it has major implications for psychoanalytic work.” Additionally, Rustin and Sekaer (2004) state, “Although some psychoanalysts still dismiss the impact of neuroscience on psychoanalysis as ‘Darwinian,’ the general view is changing.”
The two-person relational concepts reviewed in this book are written to help the reader expand, synthesize, and contextualize their applicability to a contemporary two-person relational psychodynamic psychotherapy, as it relates to children and adolescents. We seek to integrate the contributions from developmental research, neuroscience, and intersubjectivity. We emphasize that our book is strongly influenced by the works of Emde, Stern, Beebe, Lackman, Wachtel, Altman, Lyons-Ruth, and the Boston Change Process Study Group.
1.1 Making the Case for Two-Person Relational Psychodynamic Psychotherapy: A Neurodevelopmentally Informed Treatment
The book’s subtitle—“Integrating Intersubjectivity and Neuroscience”—was carefully chosen to underscore the critical neurobiological and neurodevelopmental basis of two-person relational psychotherapy. Importantly, recent advances in neuroscientific and behavioral research over the last two decades have greatly improved our understanding of relatedness, social cognition, and implicit memory. Moreover, these advances have given today’s two-person relational psychotherapist a greater understanding of “(1) the importance of early life experiences, as well as the inseparable and highly interactive influences of genetics and environment, on the development of the brain and the unfolding of human behavior; (2) the central role of early relationships as a source of either support and adaptation or risk and dysfunction; (3) that powerful capabilities, complex emotions, and essential social skills that develop during the earliest years of life; (4) the capacity to increase the odds of favorable developmental outcomes through planned interventions” (Institute of Medicine 2000). Thus, these advances in neuroscience serve as the bedrock for the work of the relationally informed psychotherapist’s practice.
Increasingly, we appreciate the role of the infant as an active participant in the reciprocal meaning-making relationship with his or her parents or caregivers and the importance of the quality of this reciprocity with regard to his or her psychological development. Inherent to this social reciprocity is the brain’s ability to process and encode complex relational experiences and to manage complexity through the selection, processing, and storing of memories that are interwoven with experiences. While the neurophysiologic processes that underlie these abilities (e.g., pruning, experience-dependent changes in functional connectivity, long-term potentiation, and neuromodulation) are discussed later in this book (Chap. 7), it is important to note that, in vivo, these processes are actively influenced and directed by relational experiences.
We hope this book provides a path of collegial communication to better understand the contemporary two-person relational psychology “from the inside,” thereby clarifying misconceptions and minimizing misunderstandings. Additionally, we hope that it will plant seeds that will germinate with understanding in the fertile minds of the trainees, newly minted child and adolescent psychiatrists, and experienced clinicians alike, who are embarking in the use of this approach in their clinical work, and create cross-fertilization among different theoretical approaches to the children and adolescents who seek our help.
Introduction to Two-Person Relational Psychology
Two-person relational psychology, unlike its forerunners in the traditional one-person psychology arena, promotes the integration of concepts from intersubjectivity, mindfulness, family systems, cognitive therapy, dialectic therapy, interpersonal therapy, and client-centered therapy, when working with patients in psychotherapy. As such, two-person relational psychology encompasses the therapeutic work that is influenced by the real-life and genuine subjective experiences between patient and psychotherapist. The psychotherapist will make use of his or her own intersubjective experience to provide nonconscious or well-timed enactments and self-disclosures to promote new adaptive patterns of interaction that occur at the implicit level and may not be open to understanding at the explicit autobiographic level (see Chap. 3). In two-person relational psychology, the psychotherapist must attend to the ever-present fact that interactions of mutuality are also influenced by context, bringing into the office the realities of the child’s world (e.g., being bullied, witnessing family conflict, difficulties with learning). Thus, in two-person relational psychology, it is no longer necessary for the psychotherapist to feel that he or she must know the patient better than the patient knows him- or herself, in order to provide insight about the origin of their maladaptive ego defenses, which is a goal in traditional one-person models.

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

