Normal Sleep

Chapter 4


Normal Sleep



4.1


Sleep in Animals and the Phylogeny of Sleep



The amount of sleep required by animals varies greatly. The horse sleeps less than 2 hours a day, and the big brown bat sleeps 20 hours a day. Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep amounts show even greater variation across species (Fig. 4.1-1). The platypus has the most REM sleep of any animal, approximately 8 hours a day (Video 4.1 and Fig. 4.1-2). Dolphins and other cetaceans do not appear to have any REM sleep, birds have very little, and conclusive evidence for the presence of REM sleep in reptiles is lacking.




Recent findings undermine the idea that sleep has a vital universal neural or physiologic function across species. Dolphins and other cetaceans never have high-voltage slow waves in both sides of the brain, in contrast to all terrestrial mammals (Fig. 4.1-3). Instead, they have unihemispheric slow waves with closure of the eye contralateral to the hemisphere with slow waves. Furthermore, killer whale and dolphin mothers and their calves are continuously active, and calves keep both eyes open for 2 or more months after birth. No rebound of inactive behavior follows. During this period, the calf’s brain and body grow to their prodigious size and capacity without any apparent need for sleep. An equally remarkable observation has been made that adult dolphins working for reward can discriminate between visual stimuli presented at 30-second intervals on their left or right sides, 24 hours a day, for as long as 15 days. During this time their performance shows no progressive decline, and no rebound of inactivity follows the continuous vigilance task. In contrast, humans whose sleep is interrupted on a similar schedule are dramatically impaired, demonstrating the variability of sleep regulation across species.



Migrating birds experience greatly reduced sleep time with intact learning abilities, high rates of performance, and no subsequent sleep rebound. The polygynous pectoral sandpiper, was shown to greatly reduce its sleep time during a 3-week mating period without signs of performance decrement or sleep rebound. Such variation in sleep time may well be typical under natural conditions. In contrast, animal (or human) studies done under safe laboratory conditions of controlled temperature and ad libitum food availability may lead to the incorrect conclusion that sleep durations are fixed.


People of similar intelligence, age, sex, and body build can have very different sleep times. They also vary in their response to sleep loss: some are highly impaired and some are unable to resist sleep, whereas others show high levels of functioning despite sleep loss. The effect of sleep deprivation on performance is not strongly related to baseline sleep duration. Furthermore, human sleep duration is not linearly related to health; both high and low values are linked to shortened life span. Some evidence suggests that individuals with greater than normal spontaneous sleep durations are at greater risk than those with less than average sleep time. A report that sleep deprivation by the “disk over water” technique leads to death in rats may be related to the stress of frequent awakenings rather than sleep loss. Sleep deprivation has not been reported to cause death in mice or rats deprived by other techniques. Fatal familial insomnia and sleeping sickness can cause death in humans, but sleep loss or excess does not appear to be responsible; these diseases affect many body organs.


Numerous attempts have been made to correlate the variation in sleep time across species with physiologic variables such as body mass, life span, brain size, brain/body weight ratio, and litter size. However, such studies have not identified correlations that account for a significant amount of the cross-species variance (Fig. 4.1-4 and Fig. 4.1-5). The few weak and inconsistent correlations that have been reported appear to be largely a function of the way the data are handled and which animals are excluded from the dataset. The definitive study of the phylogeny of REM and non-REM (NREM) sleep times in birds concluded that none of the physiologic parameters typically studied in mammals showed even a weak relationship with sleep time. However, one relation appears to be consistent across both birds and mammals: species that eat food with low caloric density, such as herbivores, sleep significantly less than those that eat more nutritionally dense foods (carnivores; see Fig. 4.1-5). In other words, if a species needs to eat more than 12 hours a day, it cannot sleep more than 12 hours; animals have evolved to adjust sleep time appropriately to such waking needs. Most species, including humans, can reduce sleep to acquire food, avoid predation, mate, be available to their young during critical periods, and to deal with other needs.




It does not appear to be the case that sleep intensity—as measured by electroencephalogram (EEG) synchrony, REM sleep time, or lack of responsiveness—is negatively correlated with sleep time. In other words, the horse and giraffe, which sleep 2 to 4 hours a day, do not sleep more deeply than the lion, which sleeps 12 to 14 hours a day. In the same way, elderly humans, who sleep 6 to 7 hours a day, do not sleep more deeply than teenagers, who sleep more than 10 hours a day. To the contrary, both developmentally and phylogenetically, the general tendency is for short sleepers to sleep less deeply than long sleepers.


So why do most of us feel so poorly when we reduce sleep? Natural selection has imposed a certain amount of sleep on us to restrict activity to appropriate times of day and to reduce long-term nonvital energy expenditure. The pressure to sleep operates by reducing brain activity. Although individuals with naturally short sleep are not at elevated risk of death compared with those who have naturally long sleep times, repeated sleep deprivation below the body’s programmed level is stressful and is likely to have significant health consequences. Certain hormonal processes are linked to sleep; these are not universal, but rather are species specific.


It has been argued that quiet waking could serve the energy conservation functions attributed to sleep without the risks associated with the sleep state. However, the brain consumes as much as 25% of the body’s energy at rest. This amount does not greatly differ between active and quiet waking, but it is greatly reduced in sleep. Animals with safe sleeping sites will achieve a selective advantage in reducing brain energy consumption by sleep. Animals with unsafe sleep sites do not sleep deeply.


Species whose environment has a severe seasonal variation in food availability have evolved to increase sleep during periods of food shortage and decrease sleep when food is available. Still others, who have safe sites, hibernate during periods of greatly reduced availability, achieving even more reduction in energy expenditure.



Selected Readings



Jones, SG, Paletz, EM, Obermeyer, WH, Hannan, CT, Benca, RM. Seasonal influences on sleep and executive function in the migratory white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii). BMC Neurosci. 2010; 11:87.


Kripke, DF, Garfinkel, L, Wingard, DL, Klauber, MR, Marler, MR. Mortality associated with sleep duration and insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002; 59:131–136.


Lesku, JA, Rattenborg, NC, Valcu, M, et al. Adaptive sleep loss in polygynous pectoral sandpipers. Science. 2012; 337:1654–1658.


Lyamin, O, Pryaslova, J, Lance, V, Siegel, J. Animal behaviour: continuous activity in cetaceans after birth. Nature. 2005; 435:1177.


Ridgway, S, Keogh, M, Carder, D, et al. Dolphins maintain cognitive performance during 72 to 120 hours of continuous auditory vigilance. J Exp Biol. 2009; 212:1519–1527.


Roth, TC, Lesku, JA, Amlaner, CJ, Lima, SL. A phylogenetic analysis of the correlates of sleep in birds. J Sleep Res. 2006; 15:395–402.


Siegel, JM. Clues to the functions of mammalian sleep. Nature. 2005; 437:1264–1271.


Siegel, JM. Do all animals sleep? Trends Neurosci. 2008; 31:208–213.


Siegel, JM. Sleep viewed as a state of adaptive inactivity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009; 10:747–753.



4.2


Normal Sleep in Humans



All organisms have periods of activity and inactivity. This is true from viruses to the most complex of mammals. Such a pervasive finding suggests that sleep is basic to life (Box 4.2-1). This chapter reviews normal sleep, the function and neurophysiology of the evolution of sleep, and the ontogeny of sleep.





Neurophysiology of Sleep and Wakefulness


The presumed function of rapid eye movement (REM), non-REM (NREM), and awake states is provided in Figure 4.2-1. Figure 4.2-2 describes the neurotransmitters involved in wakefulness-generating neural networks, which include glutamate, acetylcholine, norepinephrine, dopamine, histamine, hypocretin, and serotonin, and provides the function and location of each. The interaction of the neuromediators of sleep on brainstem structures responsible for arousal and sleep is described in Figures 4.2-3 and 4.2-4.






image


Figure 4.2-4 The brain in the awake state illustrates important arousal and sleep centers and pathways of neurotransmission.
A, Cholinergic input (orange) from the laterodorsal tegmental (LDT) and pedunculopontine (PPT) nuclei project through the thalamus and facilitate thalamocortical transmission of arousal signals. A second pathway projects through the hypothalamus to cortical centers and facilitates the processing of thalamocortical inputs that arise from midbrain centers, including the noradrenergic (blue) locus coeruleus (LC); the serotonergic (purple) dorsal raphe (Raphe); the histaminergic (pink) tuberomammillary nucleus (TMN); and the dopaminergic (yellow) ventral periaqueductal grey matter (VPAG). This pathway also receives input from the cholinergic (orange) basal forebrain (BF) and the peptidergic neurons of the lateral hypothalamus (LH) and perifornical neurons (PeF), which contain hypocretin or melanin-concentrating hormone (light green). The melatonergic (red) neural network affects arousal and sleep through regulation of circadian rhythms. This internal biologic clock originates in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), projects through the dorsomedial hypothalamus (DMH), and sends inhibitory signals to the γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-ergic (grey) ventrolateral preoptic nucleus (VLPO) of the hypothalamus. B, Artistic rendering of the human brain in the sleeping state illustrates important sleep and arousal centers and pathways of neurotransmission. The VLPO of the hypothalamus sends descending GABAergic inhibitory signals to the midbrain arousal centers that include the PeF, TMN, VPAG, raphe, LDT and PPT, and LC. During the early hours of dark periods, the pineal gland (Pin) releases melatonin (red), which has inhibitory effects on the SCN and DMH of the melatonergic system. Nuclei that control neural activity during REM sleep have been identified in the pontine midbrain. The pericoeruleus (PC) and parabranchial (PB) nuclei send glutaminergic (green) projections through the BF to affect cortical activity during REM sleep, and projections from the sublaterodorsal nucleus (SLD) send glutamatergic signals through the spinal cord to induce the atonia characteristic of REM sleep. (From Wafford KA, Bjarke E: Emerging anti-insomnia drugs: tackling sleeplessness and the quality of wake time. Nat Rev Drug Disc 2008;7:530–540.)



Wakefulness


Wakefulness is promoted by the ascending brain arousal systems; inhibition of these systems promotes sleep (Fig. 4.2-5). The brainstem reticular activating system (RAS) contains neurons that inhibit sleep-generating neurons and activate cortical neurons through ascending projections via the thalamus, hypothalamus, and basal forebrain. Descending projections from the reticular formation to the spinal cord are important for maintaining postural control and muscle tone.



image


Figure 4.2-5 Hypothalamic mechanisms in sleep-wake control.
Importance of wakefulness- and sleep-promoting systems in the hypothalamus has been recently emphasized. These systems innervate the ascending arousal systems (see Fig. 4.2-1) and excite and inhibit the system, respectively, to mediate the effects. A, Hypocretin neurons in the lateral hypothalamic area innervate all the ascending arousal systems as well as the cerebral cortex. B, Neurons of the ventrolateral preoptic (VLPO) area produce γ-aminobutyric acid and galanin, which inhibit all the arousal systems during non–rapid eye movement sleep. BF, basal forebrain; LC, locus coeruleus; LDT, laterodorsal tegmental nuclei; PPT, pedunculopontine; SN, substantia nigra; TMN, tuberomammillary nucleus; VTA, ventral tegmental area. (From Basic sleep concepts, science, deprivation, and mechanisms of neurotransmitters and neuropharmacology of sleep/wake regulations. In Nishino S [ed]: Encyclopedia of sleep. Palo Alto, CA, 2013, Stanford University School of Medicine, pp 395–406. Accessed online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-378610-4.00087-5.)



Transition Between Wakefulness and Sleep


Key physiologic changes that occur during the transition from wakefulness into sleep are shown in Figures 4.2-6 through 4.2-8.



image


Figure 4.2-6 In mammals, the two types of sleep are rapid eye movement (REM) and non-REM (NREM).
These sleep types are defined in terms of electrophysiologic signs detected with a combination of electroencephalography (EEG), electrooculography (EOG), and electromyography (EMG), the measurement of which in humans is collectively termed polysomnography. REM sleep—also known as paradoxical, active, or desynchronized sleep—is characterized by awakelike and “activated” (high-frequency, low-amplitude, or “desynchronized”) signals in the EEG; singlets and clusters of REMs in the EOG; and very low muscle tone (atonia) in the EMG. Note that the term desynchronized for the activated states of waking and REM has been rendered obsolete by the discovery of highly synchronized γ-frequency (30 to 80 Hz) activity in these states. NREM sleep is divided into four stages that correspond to increasing depth of sleep, as indicated by progressive dominance of the EEG by high-voltage, low-frequency “synchronized” wave activity. Such low-frequency waves dominate the deepest stages of NREM (stages 3 and 4, also termed slow-wave sleep). Stage 2 NREM is characterized by distinctive sleep spindles, K-complex waveforms, and a slow (<1 Hz) oscillation, which influences their timing. A shows the characteristic waveforms of the different sleep stages. Changes in peripheral physiology associated with the sleep stages is shown in C. NREM and REM sleep alternate in each of the four or five cycles that occur in each night of adult human sleep. Early in the night, NREM sleep is deeper and occupies a disproportionately large amount of time, especially in the first cycle, when the REM epoch may be short or aborted. Later in the night, NREM sleep is shallow, and more of each cycle is devoted to REM (red bars). B illustrates these changes over the course of a night’s sleep. A depicts, in detail, features of an early-night sleep cycle in which NREM reaches its greatest depth at stages 3 and 4 (delta-sleep), whereas C depicts a late-night cycle in which NREM descends only to stage 3. The constant period length of the NREM-REM cycle indicates that it is timed by a reliable oscillator, the amplitude of which varies according to extrinsic factors. The cyclic organization of sleep varies within and among species. The period length of each REM-NREM epoch increases with brain size across species, and the depth and proportion of the NREM phase in each cycle increases with brain maturation within species. NREM sleep complexity is a function of brain systems, such as the thalamocortical circuitry, that reach their maximum development in mature humans only to decline in postmature age. It can therefore be concluded that the differentiation of sleep is a function of brain differentiation, a rule that indicates both mechanistic and functional links between sleep and other brain functions. (From Pace-Schott EF, Hobson JA: The neurobiology of sleep: genetics, cellular physiology and subcortical networks. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3:591–605.)

< div class='tao-gold-member'>

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Jul 11, 2016 | Posted by in NEUROLOGY | Comments Off on Normal Sleep

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access