William T. O’Donohue, Lorraine T. Benuto and Lauren Woodward Tolle (eds.)Handbook of Adolescent Health Psychology201310.1007/978-1-4614-6633-8_16© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013
School Performance in Adolescence
(1)
School of Education, Massey University, 102904, North Shore City, Auckland, 0745, New Zealand
(2)
68 Nautilus Drive, Gulf Harbour, Auckland, 0930, New Zealand
Abstract
Schools are the one location where virtually all children and youth are present. High school graduation, the culmination of an individual’s high school education, is important with regard to the individual’s future earnings and the economy of the entire country. School performance (grades) and attendance are the two most critical variables affecting high school graduation. In spite of that, multiple factors can influence school performance in adolescence. These include disability, self-regulation, social and emotional functioning, chronic illness, exposure to trauma, and factors such as obesity, pregnancy, bullying, or a combination of these. This chapter reviewed each of these with respect to their influence on school performance.
Adolescence is a time of transition that often involves decreases in academic achievement or perceptions of academic competence (Fredricks & Eccles, 2002; Ryan & Patrick, 2001). For example, Fredricks and Eccles studied beliefs of competence in math and found consistent declines in math ability perceptions across the school years. They speculate that this may be due to young children tending to be overly optimistic with regard to their competence and having less comparative standards with which to judge their competence than adolescents. It is entirely possible that adolescents are more accurate and critical in their assessment of their abilities than are younger children. They also note that elementary school teachers are more likely to use criterion-referenced testing while middle and high school students are faced with a greater range of norm-referenced assessment measures which allows them to become better judges of their ability relative to their peers. Is such a perception accurate however?
If one listened only to public media outlets, one may get the conclusion that the American public education system is broken, students are not learning much, and high school graduates are attaining less than those attending school in previous years. The data do not support that conclusion, however. The National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) is a continuing and nationally representative measure of achievement in various subjects over time (U.S. Department of Education, 2008a). The trends in NEAP performance over the past four decades indicate a steady increasing trend in math performance for 9- and 13-year-olds and a consistent performance across time for 17-year-olds (U.S. Department of Education, 2008b). Results in reading performance were similar with an increasing trend noted for 9-year-olds and consistent performance for 13- and 17-year-olds. It should be noted, however, that the average score for the 2008 sample of students was higher for both reading and math at each age level than in the initial year of data collection (1971 for Reading, 1973 for Math).
School Attendance
To paraphrase Yogi Berra, school success is “90 percent showing up; the other half is mental” (Reeve, 2008, p. 90). In spite of the attempted humor of the remark, there is no question that a lack of consistent school attendance is a serious concern. Lack of consistent attendance in school is a major barrier to academic achievement which can lead to poor mental health (Goldstein, Little, & Akin-Little, 2003; Gottfried, 2010). Overall, American students with higher rates of attendance perform better on standardized achievement tests and have lower rates of dropping out of school, are less likely to engage in delinquent or destructive behaviors (Sheldon, 2007), and are more likely to graduate from high school (Gewertz, 2007). To that end, the No Child Left Behind legislation (NCLB, 2002) included attendance as one of the criteria in a school’s evaluation for adequate yearly progress.
High School Graduation
Maintaining a high percentage of high school graduates is an important part of a nation’s economy (Goldstein et al., 2003). Individuals who fail to graduate from high school can have an enduring effect on the economy because they are more likely to be unqualified to perform duties in an increasing technical and service-related economy. Graduation from high school is accomplished by successfully passing specific classes and, in most states, a state exit exam. Nationally, only 78 % of white students, 56 % of African American students, and 52 % of Hispanic students graduate from high school 4 years after they enter 9th grade (Greene, 2009). The states with the worst high school graduation rates are Georgia, which graduated only 54 % of its students in 4 years, Nevada (58 %), and Florida, Arizona, and the District of Columbia (all 59 %) (Greene). Additionally, students in predominantly African American and Latino schools are less likely to earn a high school diploma or equivalent (Goldsmith, 2009). Attendance is a major factor in high school graduation success (Pinkus, 2009) because when children and youth are in school, they have the opportunity to succeed academically, an opportunity not afforded to them when not in attendance. A study by the Consortium on Chicago School Research indicated that 9th-grade grades and attendance are two of the most important indicators of high school graduation (Gewertz, 2007).
Individuals with Disabilities
School performance can be impacted by several factors, including disabilities. Federal legislation has, since the early 1970s, guaranteed the rights of all children and youth in the United States to a “Free Appropriate Public Education” regardless of the presence of any disability. Prior to this time, many children and youth were denied access to education simply because they had a disability. For example, in 1970, US schools educated only one in five children with disabilities, and many states had laws excluding certain students, including children who were deaf, blind, emotionally disturbed, or mentally retarded (U.S. Department of Education, 2007). Current legislation (e.g., IDEA), however, mandates that schools provide appropriate educational opportunities for these children.
The first legislation addressing the rights of the disabled was Section 504 of the Rehabilitation Act of 1973. This law stated that recipients of federal assistance could not discriminate on the basis of a handicap. As all public schools receive federal assistance, the tenets of this law apply to schools. One specific part of Section 504 mandates a “Free Appropriate Public Education” to qualified people with a disability, regardless of the nature or severity of the disability. This was the first use of this term in federal legislation. This law was expanded in 1990 with the passage of the Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA).
Congress enacted the landmark Education for All Handicapped Children Act (Public Law 94-142), in 1975. This legislation insured the right to public education for all persons, including the disabled. It included four major provisions: (1) that all children and youth have available to them a free appropriate public education which emphasizes special education and related services designed to meet their unique needs, (2) that the rights of children and youth with disabilities and their parents are protected, (3) that the federal government will assist states and localities to provide for the education of all children and youth with disabilities, and (4) to assess and assure the effectiveness of efforts to educate all children with disabilities.
This law was amended in 1986 (PL 99-457) at which time early intervention services were authorized. At the time of the 1990 reauthorization, the name of the act was changed to the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA). Further reauthorizations with modifications occurred in 1997 and 2005. There are 13 disability categories specified in IDEA. They are:
Autism: A developmental disability significantly affecting verbal and nonverbal communication and social interaction, generally evident before age 3
Deafness: A hearing impairment that is so severe that the student is impaired in processing linguistic information through hearing, with or without amplification
Deaf-blindness: Simultaneous hearing and visual impairments
Emotional disturbance: A disability whereby a student of typical intelligence has difficulty, over time and to a marked degree, building satisfactory interpersonal relationships; responds inappropriately behaviorally or emotionally under normal circumstances; demonstrates a pervasive mood of unhappiness; or has a tendency to develop physical symptoms or fears
Hearing impairment: An impairment in hearing, whether permanent or fluctuating, that is not included under deafness
Mental retardation: Significant subaverage general intellectual functioning existing concurrently with deficits in adaptive behavior and manifested during the developmental period
Multiple disabilities: The manifestation of two or more disabilities (e.g., mental retardation-blindness), the combination of which requires special accommodation for maximal learning
Orthopedic impairment: Physical disabilities, including congenital impairments (e.g., club foot), impairments caused by disease (e.g., poliomyelitis), and impairments from other causes (e.g., cerebral palsy)
Other health impairment: Having limited strength, vitality, or alertness due to chronic or acute health problems (e.g., asthma, heart condition, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder)
Specific learning disability: A disorder in one or more of the basic psychological processes involved in understanding or in using language, spoken or written, which may manifest itself in an imperfect ability to listen, think, speak, read, write, spell, or do mathematical calculations
Speech or language impairment: A communication disorder such as stuttering, impaired articulation, a language impairment, or a voice impairment
Traumatic brain injury: An acquired injury to the brain caused by an external physical force or by a certain medical condition (e.g., brain tumor) resulting in total or partial functional disability or psychosocial impairment, or both
Visual impairment: A visual difficulty (including both partial sight and blindness) that, even with correction, adversely affects a student’s educational performance
Factors Related to School Performance in Adolescence
Self-Regulation
Self-regulation has been defined as an individual’s ability to control and regulate his/her own actions, cognitions, and emotions as well as the ability to begin and end activities based on situational demands, and to modulate their behavior in social and educational settings (Bronson, 2000). Self-regulation is believed to develop during adolescence and by the age of 13 or 14, adolescents generally develop the ability to be planful with regard to their short- and long-term goals and thus able to regulate their behavior to increase the likelihood that these goals will be attained (Demetriou, 2000). Research suggests that students who engage in self-regulatory behavior do better in school (Pintrich, 2000) and that students who observe and evaluate their performance accurately can react appropriately by keeping or changing their study strategies to achieve optimal results (Hartman, 2001). Bakracevic Vukman and Licardo (2010) examined the relationship between self-regulation and school achievement. Results indicated that metacognitive self-regulation was found to be an important predictor of school achievement in both primary and secondary school students, with self-regulation explaining 21 % of the variance of school performance their secondary school sample.
The process of self-regulation includes three subprocesses: self-monitoring, self-evaluation, and behavioral adjustment (Singer & Bashir, 1999). The question then arises: can self-regulation skills be improved via directed interventions? Research has consistently demonstrated that direct instruction, modeling, and practice and feedback have all been found effective in teaching students self-monitoring and self-regulation procedures which have subsequently been found to improve both academic performance and classroom behavior (Coleman & Webber, 2002; Zimmerman, 1996).
Social and Emotional Functioning: Depression and Conduct Disorder
Two psychological disorders which can have a major impact on both academic achievement and graduation are depression and conduct disorder. An increase in adjustment problems, such as depression, is not uncommon during adolescent development (Graber, 2004). The rate of depression increases throughout the adolescent years, with a rate of 3.9 % for 12-year-olds, 11.6 % for 16-year-olds, and 10.6 % for 17-year-olds with the rate among girls being three times that for boys (National Institute for Mental Health, 2010). By later adolescence, the prevalence of major depression is very similar to prevalence for adults (Hankin, 2006) with 18–24-years-olds having the highest rate of overall depression of any age group (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2011).
Depression in adolescence has been found to be related to academic performance and high school graduation (Humensky et al., 2010). For example, Wilcox-Gok, Marcotte, Farahati, and Borkoski (2004) found that men who retrospectively recalled onset of depression prior to age 16 were less likely to graduate from high school. Similarly, Fletcher (2008) found that adolescent girls diagnosed with depression were less likely to graduate from high school or enroll in college. In addition, Ding, Lehrer, Rosenquist, and Audrain-McGovern (2009) found that female adolescents with depression had greater decreases in school performance than males.
Humensky and colleagues (2010) examined school performance among adolescents at risk for major depression. Results indicated that the relationship between depressive symptoms and actual school performance was not statistically significant; however, adolescent perception of functioning and school performance was related to depressive symptoms. They found that depressive symptoms affected adolescents’ perception of their ability to perform in school, do their homework, concentrate, and engage in other adaptive academic behaviors.
Fröjda et al. (2008) found, in a large sample of Finnish 7th through 9th graders, that when participants reported a lower GPA or when their academic performance had decreased from the previous term, they were more likely to self-report depressive symptoms at a level consistent with moderate to severe depression. A relationship was also found between depression and other factors such as concentration and social relationships. The study concluded that the lower a student’s GPA, the higher the likelihood of them being depressed.
Conduct-disordered (CD) youth are at an even greater risk of failure to achieve academically. That is because, by definition, children and adolescents diagnosed with CD are likely to eventually come into contact with the criminal justice system. Virtually every one of the 15 criteria listed in the DSM-IV for CD involves a behavior that, if manifested to a sufficient magnitude and chronicity, will eventually lead to arrest and possible incarceration. This generally necessitates dismissal from school or low attendance which can negatively affect both grades and graduation rate. Grisso (1998) reviewed the epidemiological research with regard to the prevalence of childhood mental disorders in delinquent populations and found that CD is the most prevalent diagnosis with a rate probably about 50–60 %.
In terms of prevalence, conduct disorder is diagnosed more frequently in males than females. A study by Keenan, Wroblewski, Hipwell, Loeber, and Stouthamer-Loeber (2010) indicated 2–16 % of boys and 1–9 % of girls may be diagnosed. Age of onset is an additional important consideration. However, presently, data are somewhat sparse regarding developmental patterns particularly for females. Some authors have suggested a need to consider differing criteria for girls in terms of overt versus covert aggression in the diagnosis of CD (Delligatti, Akin-Little, & Little, 2003).
Kazdin (1998) and Sholevar and Sholevar (1995) both discuss the correlation between school-related factors and CD. These factors include attending school where little emphases is placed on academic work, the poor physical condition of the school building(s), infrequent use of teacher praise, low teacher expectancy, and teacher unavailability to deal with students’ problems. Given the lack of promise to both scholastic achievement and graduation for students diagnosed with CD (Boden, Fergusson, & Horwood, 2010), all studies emphasize the critical nature of early, wraparound (i.e., inclusion of school and familial environment), efficacious intervention (e.g., Murray & Farrington, 2010).
Finally, Crick and Dodge (1994) present a reformulated social information-processing (SIP) model that can help explain the development of aggressive and antisocial behavior in school-age children. They proposed that children process social information in six steps: (1) encoding cues, (2) interpreting cues, (3) clarifying goals, (4) accessing or constructing responses, (5) deciding on responses, and (6) enacting behaviors. Research has consistently supported links between this model and overt aggressive behavior, particularly in boys (Crick & Dodge, 1996; Dodge et al., 2003). In addition, social rejection, as predicted from this model, has been found to reduce inhibit classroom participation (Ladd, 2008) and increase antisocial behavior (Martens & Witt, 2004).
Obesity
Obesity is usually defined as having a body mass index (BMI) of 30 or greater (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2010). Further the CDC has identified obesity rates in the United States ranging from a low of 18.6 % of the population in Colorado to a high of 34.4 % of the population in Mississippi with African Americans and Hispanics both having a higher rate of obesity than Whites (51 % and 21 % higher, respectively) with similar patterns found in children and adolescents (Ogden, Carroll, Curtin, Lamb, & Flegal, 2010). Sabia (2007) found a consistent negative relationship between BMI and grade point average (GPA) for white adolescent females. In a review of research on the association between obesity and academic performance in school-age children, Taras and Potts-Datema (2005) concluded being overweight or obese was associated with lower academic achievement; however, the directionality of this relationship is not clear.
Studies have shown that there is also a relationship between obesity and low self-esteem and mental health conditions such as anxiety and depression (Zametkin, Zoon, Klein, & Munson, 2004). It is entirely possible that it is these conditions that predispose children and adolescents to both obesity and poor school performance (Datar, Sturm, & Magnabosso, 2004). However, in a study of Korean adolescents, Cho, Lambert, Kim, and Kim (2009) concluded from their data that obesity and poor academic performance are simultaneously determined.
Chronic Illness
Chronic medical conditions are defined as those having debilitating symptoms or which last for more than 3 months a year (Thompson & Gustafson, 1996) and include developmental illnesses (e.g., cerebral palsy) and chronic diseases (e.g., diabetes, epilepsy). About 10.3 million children and adolescents in the United States have chronic medical conditions or illnesses, which involve limitations in cognitive, physical, or psychosocial development (Algozzine & Ysseldyke, 2006; Valletutti, 2004; Walsh & Murphy, 2003). In addition, about 4.4 million children with chronic conditions face problems hindering their participation in school (Kaffenberger, 2006; Thies & McAllister, 2001) which may interfere with their school performance (Nabors, Little, Akin-Little, & Iobst, 2008).
It is not uncommon for factors related to the chronic illness and not the illness itself to contribute to poor academic performance. Smith, Taylor, Newbould, and Keady (2008) suggested that the adverse effects of medication may be related to school performance. Calsbeek, Rijken, Bekkers, Dekker, and van Berge Henegouwen (2006) examined adolescents and young adults with chronic digestive disorders and found a relationship between the disease and academic performance. Their conclusions, however, was that it was depression associated with the disease that led to the academic difficulties and not the disease itself. McNelis, Johnson, Huberty, and Austin (2005) examined the relationship between chronic epilepsy and academic performance. While their results indicated that most students with chronic seizures perform in the average range, teachers rate 25 % as at or below one standard deviation of the mean. Similarly, Nabors and colleagues’ (2008) results indicated that relatively few teachers in their survey indicated having high knowledge of chronic illnesses or confidence in working with students with all but the most common chronic illnesses (e.g., asthma). Spilkin and Ballantyne (2007) found that while children with cystinosis, a genetic disorder characterized by the abnormal accumulation of the amino acid cystine, may have real academic limitations related to their disease, teachers do not always recognize this and misattribute it to laziness. Finally, in discussing children with cancer and academic performance, Armstrong and Horn (1995) noted that school performance may be affected by the cancer but the type and severity of the performance deficit is dependent on the type and location of the tumor. Further, cancer treatments (e.g., radiation, chemotherapy) may interfere with a child or adolescent’s ability to attend school or result in long-term cognitive deficits (e.g., processing speed) which may affect school performance.
Pregnancy
Approximately 750,000 girls and women age 15–19 become pregnant each year in the United States, with 59 % of these pregnancies resulting in birth (Guttmacher Institute, 2010), a rate nearly twice that of Canada (McKay & Barrett, 2010). Teenage childbearing has been found to have unfavorable outcomes for both mother and child (Meade, Kershaw, & Ickovics, 2008), including lower educational attainment (Coley & Chase-Lansdale, 1998). For example, Jones, Astone, Keyl, Kim, and Alexander (1999) found that teen childbearing reduces the probability of completing high school by 8–10 %.
Trauma
Unfortunately, it is not uncommon for adolescents to be exposed to traumatic events (Little, Akin-Little & Somerville, in press). Children and adolescents may experience a number of different types of trauma. These can include abuse (e.g., sexual, physical), grief, exposure to domestic and community violence, natural disasters, or a combination of the above (Little, Akin-Little, & Gutierrez, 2009). Felitti and colleagues (1998) conducted a retrospective study of over 17,000 adults and found that more than one half of their sample reported experiencing at least one adverse event in childhood with approximately one quarter reporting having experienced two or more.
While not every child exposed to a traumatic event experiences negative outcomes, trauma symptoms are not uncommon and have been categorized into four domains: affective, behavioral, cognitive, and physical (Cohen, Mannarino, & Deblinger, 2006). Common affective symptoms include fear, depression, anger, and frequent mood changes; behavioral symptoms usually center on avoiding reminders of the trauma; cognitive symptoms usually involve distorted cognitions about themselves, others, the event, or the world (e.g., “the event is my fault”); and physical symptoms include stress related responses such as elevated heart rates and blood pressure, increased muscle tension, and hypervigilance. It has also been found that prolonged exposure to trauma and maltreatment can decrease brain size and functioning (De Bellis et al., 1999). With regard to school performance, few studies exist that directly relate trauma exposure to decreases in school performance. The trauma symptoms described by Cohen and colleagues above all are likely to have an impact on school performance however. In one of the few empirical studies comparing trauma exposure to a no trauma control group, Green et al. (2001) found that a traumatic loss group had higher rates of impaired school performance compared to the no trauma control group. Similarly, in a more recent study from the UK, Abdelnoor and Hollins (2004) found significant underachievement in secondary school performance for adolescents who lost a parent to death.

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

