PTSD, Arousal, and Disrupted (REM) Sleep



Fig. 19.1
Inverted U shape describing the relationship between tonic LC activity and task performance , showing optimal performance at intermediate levels of tonic LC activity, allowing phasic LC activity in response to cued targets requiring a motor response (From Aston-Jones et al. [12], reproduced with kind permission from Elsevier)



Increased central noradrenergic concentrations have been observed in PTSD patients in wakefulness [13], as well as in 24-h urinary measures in larger samples [14]. If this is related to increased tonic LC output, PTSD patients should show impaired performance in tasks probing for attention and/or general information processing speed . This is indeed the case: a recent meta-analysis on 4108 PTSD patients, trauma-exposed controls, and healthy controls has revealed a Cohen’s d of around −0.5 for attention and working memory tasks and a Cohen’s d of around −0.6 for information processing speed (both reduced in PTSD patients) [15]. Moreover, another meta-analysis on the P3 response as measured with EEG in PTSD revealed that the P3a to trauma-related distractor stimuli was higher in PTSD patients [16]. One study has provided evidence that the (frontal) P3 to irrelevant distractor stimuli is also increased with PTSD, which would indicate higher distractibility and tonic LC activity as in Fig. 19.1, and that this was related to both hyperarousal and re-experiencing symptom clusters [17].



The LC-Noradrenalin System and Sleep


Another important role for the LC-noradrenalin system is related to the transitioning of stages within sleep: noradrenergic output of the LC is highest in wakefulness and lower in non-REM sleep and reaches its nadir in REM sleep [18]. REM sleep discontinues the moment noradrenergic LC cells (and serotonergic cells in the dorsal raphe nucleus) start firing [19], reason why the LC is also referred to as a REM off switch. It is also worth noting that firing of just 10% of neurons in LC suffices to maintain normal cortical function [20]. This suggests that also minor increases in noradrenalin levels during REM sleep, which may not even be detectable peripherally, could already have an effect on REM sleep and potentially, the SWS-REM cycles seen throughout the night.

A blunted circadian rhythm of 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol (MHPG) [21], the central metabolite of noradrenalin, has also been reported in PTSD. Regarding sleep, although nightmares and insomnia are frequently reported symptoms in PTSD [6], polysomnographic studies on PTSD (a combination of EEG, electromyography, and electrooculography) have been rather inconclusive up until recently. Inconsistencies in the data were largely resolved by a meta-analysis that controlled for confounding factors that affect sleep (gender, age, comorbid depression, and substance abuse) and demonstrated that the amount of slow-wave sleep (SWS) amount is reduced, whereas light sleep stage 1 and REM density are increased in PTSD [7]. Note, however, that effects are modest at best. The question is whether increased REM density is due to an increased number of phasic REM bursts or disrupted fragments of tonic REM sleep; but having shorter and more frequent REM sleep periods within 2 weeks after a motor vehicle accident has been reported to be a significant predictor of PTSD symptomatology months later [8]. Finally, also in line with a role for the LC-noradrenalin system in dysregulating (REM) sleep in PTSD is the relative success of the noradrenergic alpha1-antagonist prazosin. When administered before sleep, prazosin seems to have positive effects on total and REM sleep length (and nightmare amelioration) [22], although these effects have not yet been observed with polysomnographic recordings [23].

Furthermore, increased noradrenergic output may in parallel affect SWS . Using microinjections in cholinergic nuclei in the basal forebrain in rodents, Cape and Jones were able to show that noradrenalin microinjections, compared to serotonergic and Ringer’s microinjections, decreased both SWS and REM percentages (both time and transitions into this stage) while increasing wakefulness [24]. Noradrenalin caused an increase in gamma and a reduction in delta activity in the EEG, with electromyography (EMG) being similarly affected by both serotonin and noradrenalin [24].

In a comprehensive review on the role of noradrenalin in sleep mentation, Gottesmann [25] proposed that it is the near absence of noradrenalin in the forebrain, specifically the nucleus accumbens, that gives rise to the maximal dopamine release taking place in REM sleep [26], which could explain increased levels of hallucinatory mental activity typical for REM sleep. A speculation worth mentioning is that increased noradrenergic levels in this region and adjacent subcortical structures such as amygdala and hypothalamus may result in increased physiological arousal and trigger emotional memory traces. However, the role of noradrenalin heavily depends on receptor subtypes (e.g., postsynaptic α1 or β, or presynaptic – inhibitory – α2 receptor function). See Berridge et al. [18] and Broese et al. [27] for reviews on noradrenergic modulation of arousal and sleep.


A Role for Cortisol in the Relationship Between Arousal and the LC System?


Closely linked to both sleep regulation and PTSD symptomatology is the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal gland (HPA) axis that orchestrates the stress response and releases glucocorticoids into the bloodstream that provide negative feedback to the hippocampus. Corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) is released by the hypothalamus and causes an increase in adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) in the pituitary, which in turn release glucocorticoids from the adrenal gland. Peripheral cortisol is at its trough during early sleep, slowly rises during the progression of the night, peaks in a cortisol-awakening response, and declines during the day. It is of note that cortisol levels are highest in the second half of the night [28] that is richest in REM sleep.

Reduced nocturnal cortisol levels have been observed in PTSD, albeit not consistently [6]. However, there is also evidence from animal models [29] and from a study in motor vehicle victims [30] that blunted cortisol responses after trauma exposure are a risk factor for PTSD. It has been proposed that, in case of reduced cortisol responses to acute stress, the noradrenergic system consequently becomes hyperactive as a homeostatic consequence, increasing the probability of PTSD development [31]. Closer to the central processes regulated by CRH is ACTH, and increased ACTH levels during the night and the awakening response were observed in PTSD and, importantly, correlated to sleep fragmentation and reduced SWS activity [32]. Interestingly, different ACTH/cortisol ratios have been shown in PTSD throughout the circadian cycle [32]. Such variability adds to the complex process of disentangling the multiple modulations of cortisol and reconciling the opposing findings; a meta-analysis on this topic seems warranted. In the meantime, continuous blood sampling during sleep in PTSD [32] appears helpful for extracting more information on ACTH and catecholamines, which might provide more unambiguous effects.


Disrupted Sleep and Disrupted Emotional Memory Consolidation/Homeostasis


The next question is then if the disruption of REM sleep impairs the consolidation of certain memories, given the role of non-REM sleep in declarative memory consolidation [33], and/or emotional homeostasis [34]. Both animal and human models have started to experimentally test this notion by employing analogous models that probe for core disrupted processes in PTSD, i.e., fear conditioning and extinction.

Fear extinction is an associative learning process central to PTSD [35, 36]. In an experimental setting, this is modeled by pairing neutral stimuli with an aversive event (fear conditioning). Subsequently presenting these stimuli alone elicits a fear response that normally extinguishes over time (fear extinction). Extinction learning and consolidation is impaired in PTSD patients [3739]. As a result, fear conditioning and extinction has been a robust model for scientific advancement on neurocognitive mechanisms underlying PTSD in both preclinical and clinical studies, leading authoritative reviews to conclude that this is one of the most successful models for translating data from animal models to human subjects and psychiatric patients (see review by Milad et al. [40]). Moreover, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), limbic and paralimbic regions such as the amygdala, hippocampus, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) have been repeatedly demonstrated to subserve fear extinction in healthy individuals (for a systematic review, see Sehlmeyer et al. [41]). Critically, these regions show abnormal fMRI activity in PTSD patients during symptom provocation [42] and during extinction and recall of extinction [38]. The dACC (excitatory influence over amygdala) and the more ventrally located vmPFC (inhibitory influence over amygdala) activity ratio during fear conditioning and extinction , as well as recall of extinction, has been identified in an extensive review on biological studies of PTSD as a promising functional imaging marker of PTSD [43].

There is a large body of preclinical data showing that the amygdala, hippocampus, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) interact through (intracranially recorded) theta oscillations during fear conditioning and extinction [35]. Research in humans has shown abnormal event-related potentials during fear conditioning and extinction in PTSD patients with surface EEG [39], and excess of frontal midline oscillations (in vmPFC) in the theta range (4–8 Hz) during affectively loaded stimuli has recently been proposed as a novel marker of PTSD [44]. One unanswered question is whether hippocampal theta in rodents translates to hippocampal theta in humans – or to hippocampal rhythmic slow activity in the 1.5–3 Hz range as shown by Bodizs et al. [45] with intracranial foramen ovale electrodes in epilepsy patients.

Recent work has shown that sleep benefits memory consolidation [33] and that REM sleep may play an essential role in emotional memory consolidation [34]. Critically, animal studies and studies on human subjects have demonstrated that specific REM deprivation impairs fear extinction consolidation [46, 47], see Fig. 19.2. Moreover, theta coherence in REM sleep predicted subsequent fear expression at recall of conditioning in rodents [48], whereas pontine wave quality in REM sleep in rodents was highly correlated with subsequent extinction memory recall [49]. This makes REM sleep, characterized by theta oscillations and pontine waves (or the human equivalents), relevant for plasticity [50], an important brain state for PTSD.

A216842_1_En_19_Fig2_HTML.gif


Fig. 19.2
Relevance of REM sleep deprivation for fear extinction consolidation . (a) In rats, REM sleep deprivation (RSD) immediately after extinction learning (0–6 h, red arrow) impaired extinction consolidation manifest in increased freezing to light stimuli that acted as the extinguished stimulus. This was not seen in the control group (C), and REM sleep deprivation later after extinction learning (6–12 h, orange arrow, different groups) did not have the same effects (From Fu et al. [46], reproduced with kind permission from Elsevier). (b) A whole night of REM sleep deprivation (blue arrows) in young human subjects specifically affected the consolidation of the extinguished stimulus in the REM sleep deprivation (REMD) group, but not of the safety stimulus (or the unextinguished stimulus – not depicted). Note that the trials per task were a priori binned into blocks to reduce analysis flexibility and that a group × time interaction over all blocks had a trend for significance in this small pilot study, suggesting rather large effect sizes (From Spoormaker et al. [47], reproduced with kind permission from John Wiley and Sons)


Quantitative EEG and Neuroimaging in PTSD Patients


The reduction in delta sleep and increase in REM density (and the tendency for REM fragmentation) in PTSD patients are in accord with heightened arousal during sleep; however quantitative EEG findings have not been unequivocal in this regard. High-frequency beta (16–30 Hz) and gamma (>40 Hz) can be taken as indices of central arousal [51], but the handful studies conducted on this topic have shown conflicting results, varying from increased to decreased beta and increased to decreased delta (for a review, see [2]). Quantitative EEG may not be optimal to evaluate group differences due to either an increased amount of artifacts (e.g., different EMG tone in PTSD) or to the fact that differences may be more subcortically pronounced. Moreover, arousals may not constitute a single entity with a well-defined neural pathway; intracranial work in epileptic patients has shown that the neural correlates of arousals widely differ dependent on sleep stage and whether they were spontaneous or nociceptive induced [52]. The thalamus showed more stereotypical responses, but these were related to different patterns of cortical arousals [52].

Other neuroimaging approaches may also be helpful to characterize sleep and understand the neurobiology of REM disruptions in sleep in PTSD patients. In a pilot study employing positron-emission tomography, Ebdlahad et al. observed widespread activity increases in REM sleep in PTSD patients compared to depressed patients, among others, in thalamus, limbic, and paralimbic circuitry [53]. In wakefulness, depressed patients showed increased metabolism in several subcortical and (para)limbic regions. Interestingly, the dorsal anterior cingulate was increased during both wakefulness and REM sleep in PTSD compared to depressed patients [53]. In a small proof-of-concept study, we showed that EEG/fMRI data of subjects falling asleep were also feasible to acquire PTSD patients (N = 6 in both the PTSD group and in the trauma-exposed control group), with extra care to be taken for EEG data quality given a seemingly high number of arousals and EMG events in both groups (Vermetten, van Liempt et al. unpublished data).

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Feb 25, 2018 | Posted by in PSYCHOLOGY | Comments Off on PTSD, Arousal, and Disrupted (REM) Sleep

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access