Range of Outcomes and Challenges in Middle and Later Life


Study

Country

Sample type

N

Diagnosis

Mean age

IQ

Social functioning composite

Very/good

Fair

Very/poor

Rutter and Lockyer (1967)

UK

Clinical

63

Infantile psychosis

15; 7

Child: 64.5

14

25

61

Lotter (1974)

UK

Epidemiologic

32

Autism

16–18

Child: 71 (range = 55–90)

14

24

62

Gillberg and Steffenburg (1987)

Sweden

Epidemiologic

24

Infantile autism

20

Child: 74 % <50

48

48

 4

Kobayashi et al. (1992)

Japan

Clinical

201

Autism (DSM-III)

21.8 (3.6 SD)

Child: 75 % ID

27

27

46

Howlin et al. (2000)

UK

Clinical

19

Autism

23.9 (1.8 SD)

Child: NVIQ 92–93

16

11

74

Engstrom et al. (2003)

Sweden

Cross-sectional

16

Asperger, high-functioning autism

30.8 (8.3 SD)

Adult: >70

12

75

12

Howlin et al. (2004)

UK

Clinical

68

Autism

29.33 (8.0 SD)

Child: NVIQ 80.2 (19 SD)

23

19

58

Billstedt et al. (2005) and Cederlund et al. (2008)

Sweden

Epidemiologic

114

Autism, atypical autism

25.5 (range 17–40)

Adult: 71 % with severe ID

 0

21

78

Eaves and Ho (2008)

Canada

Clinical

48

ASD

24 (range 19–31)

Child: 83 % VIQ <70, 49 % NVIQ < 50

21

32

47

Farley et al. (2009)

USA

Epidemiologic

41

Autism

32.5 (5.7 SD)

Adult: 88.93 (25.7 SD)

48

34

17

Marriage et al. (2009)

Canada

Clinical

80

ASD

19–55

Adult: 16 % with ID

10



Gillespie-Lynch et al. (2012)

USA

Research sample

20

Autism

26.6 (3.8 SD)

Child: 54.7 (15.5 SD)

30

20

50



In one of the earliest studies of post-childhood outcome for adults with ASD (termed “infantile psychosis” in the original publication), Rutter et al. (1967) examined adolescent outcomes for 63 children who had been identified between 1950 and 1958 through clinical and educational programs at the Maudsley Hospital Children’s Department. About one-fifth of the sample experienced a developmental regression in early childhood. The sample included children at all levels of intellectual ability, with 43 % having severe intellectual disability, and 29 % having IQs in the near-normal or normal ranges. Like other studies from this period, half of the participants were institutionalized at the time of the outcome assessments. The investigators noted prognosis for these individuals was poor as only 17 % could be “said to be well adjusted.” One person was described as having “normal” adult functioning, and eight more were doing relatively well in regard to achieving some independence in adulthood. Outcomes were Poor or Very Poor for 61 % of the sample. Unlike children today whose access to public education is protected by legislation, 21 of the individuals in this sample never attended any school at all, and less than half of the children had as much as 2 years of formal schooling.

Lotter (1974) followed 32 individuals who were identified through an epidemiologic survey in Middlesex, England when they were 8–10 years old. The mean IQ for these individuals in childhood was 71, with a range of 55–90. Eight years later, one person had passed away, and two were lost to follow-up. Sixty-two percent of those remaining were described as requiring “extensive care and supervision.” Outcomes were rated as Good for 14 % and Poor or Very Poor for 60 %. Half were institutionalized. Five of the individuals in the Very Poor outcome category had been excluded from any educational services as they grew up, and Lotter reported that individuals’ placements over time, whether in schools or institutions, were associated with their outcome status.

A decade passed before another outcome study was published (Rumsey et al., 1985). This retrospective study concerned 14 men with a mean age of 28 who were recruited through a national search for eligible adult males with DSM-III autistic disorder in the US. Individuals with seizure disorders, identifiable causal medical conditions, or who were unable to discontinue current medications were excluded. The men were studied intensively in an inpatient setting over 5 days. Nine were “unusually highly functioning” with nonverbal IQ scores above 80 and well-developed language abilities. Stereotyped movements were observed on the unit in 86 % of the participants. Basic achievement was commensurate with measured IQ scores, but measures of adaptive functioning were far below expected, given the men’s IQs, which is a robust finding from subsequent ASD research. Only one man was in a state institution.

Gillberg and Steffenburg (1987) studied outcome for a population-based sample of 23 people with ASD in late adolescence or early adulthood. As children, one-third obtained IQ scores in the mildly intellectually impaired range, and 26 % achieved scores in the normal or near-normal ranges. One-third had communicative speech at age 6. One person achieved a Good outcome and 44 % had Poor or Very Poor outcomes. Childhood IQ and use of communicative speech at age 6 were useful predictors of outcome status.

Kobayashi et al. (1992) conducted a follow-up investigation of 201 adolescents and adults identified with ASD in childhood through clinical services in Japan. Four of the people had died. The mean age for the remaining 197 young adults was 21. About one-fourth of the sample had an IQ score of 70 or better at age 6, and about 20 % were able to speak without echolalia at that age. Forty percent of the sample began school in a general education class, but only 27 % remained in general education at the age of 12. Outcome adjustment for roughly one-fourth of the sample was Good or better, and was Poor or Very Poor for 46 %. Childhood IQ was the only strong predictor of outcome in this investigation. Although there were similarities between the sample in this study and others reported, the outcome for these participants was better than in previous studies. The authors provided some possible explanations including sociodemographic factors in Japan, advances in public education standards for people with disabilities, intensive intervention histories, and a high proportion of people with ASD and average-range IQ scores at baseline.

In 1996, Ballaban-Gil, Rapin, Tuchman, and Shinnar reported on their study of outcome in 54 adolescents and 45 adults with ASD after an 18-year period. The follow-up procedure consisted of a 30-min telephone interview with caregivers for the members of the sample. Three people had died at the time of the follow-up. Two were described as no longer having any social deficits. Behavioral difficulties were reported for 69 % of the sample, and 40 % of the adults were prescribed medications aimed at controlling behavior. Thirteen adults were described as high-functioning.

Howlin, Mawhood, and Rutter (2000) compared outcomes for 19 men diagnosed in childhood with autism. Participants were 7–8 years old at the time of the childhood assessment and were identified through their involvement in hospitals or special school programs in the community. They were 23 years, 9 months old on average at the time of the adult assessment. Roughly three-fourths of the men continued to exhibit severe social difficulties in adulthood. Only one-fourth was rated as exhibiting minimal or no “autistic-type behaviors.” Just over half of the men relied on others to schedule and organize leisure activities for them, and one-third was described as having no or very limited interests or leisure activities. Three-fourths of the sample experienced a Poor or Very Poor outcome and 16 % experienced a Good outcome or better. Analyses of childhood variables that were associated with adult functioning indicated that early language skills for these men were highly related to social functioning in adulthood.

In 2003, Engstrom, Ekstrom, and Emilsson reported on the outcome for 42 Swedish adults identified through public health service records with Asperger’s disorder or so-called high-functioning autism. Eligible participants were over 18 years of age and had adult IQ scores of 70 or higher. Participants included 24 men and 18 women ranging in age from 18 to 49 years. Ten individuals had autism diagnoses, and 32 had Asperger’s disorder diagnoses. The participants with Asperger’s disorder had been diagnosed at a mean age of 28 compared to age 13 for the group with autism. From among the full sample, 16 were selected as a representative subsample that included people from both diagnostic groups, genders, and a range of ages. Participants with ASD and care providers responded to in-depth interviews designed to obtain a picture of the adult situation for these men and women. Only one adult required no support from others. Almost all participants were supported through pensions from the state. While virtually all of the sample required a high level of support, the authors noted this was a selected sample of adults with ASD who had been identified through public service records as having an ASD.

Howlin et al. (2004) studied adult outcome for 68 people with ASD who also had a childhood nonverbal IQ score of 50 or better. The mean age at the initial evaluation was 7 and at follow-up was 29 years. Average nonverbal IQ scores in adulthood were slightly below childhood scores. Almost all of the subjects were known to have attended compulsory schooling; however, only 22 % left school having achieved formal qualifications. At the time of the follow-up investigation, 23 people were employed. Eight worked in regular, independent jobs; one was self-employed as an artist but was unable to earn a living wage; and 14 worked in sheltered or supported employment. Twenty-seven people were occupied in general work/leisure programs at day centers for adults with disabilities. Outcome adjustment ratings for the sample included Good or Very Good outcomes for one-fourth of the sample and Poor or Very Poor outcomes for roughly 60 %. Childhood IQ was a useful predictor of adult adjustment in that those with childhood nonverbal IQ scores of 70 or more functioned more independently than those with scores below 70. A score of 100 or better did not increase the likelihood that a person would do well in adulthood. For those capable of completing a childhood verbal IQ measure, the combination of verbal and nonverbal IQ scores in childhood provided a more precise indication of outcome classification, with scores above 70 in both domains yielding the greatest likelihood of a Fair outcome or better. Language level at age 5 was useful in predicting overall outcome and residential status, but none of the other outcome variables studied had predictive utility.

Billstedt et al. (2005) followed up with members of three population-based studies of autism in Sweden, for a total sample of 120, after a period of 13–22 years. Participants ranged in age from 17 to 40 years and included 84 men and 36 women. Six participants had died at the time of the follow-up assessment, and only six participants from the original sample declined to participate. Outcomes were rated as Poor or Very Poor for 78 %. No participant was rated as having an outcome better than Fair. Only four adults were independent, and these individuals were described as “leading fairly isolated lives.” Sixty-two of 73 participants examined for current diagnostic status continued to meet criteria for autistic disorder, and one person no longer met diagnostic criteria for any ASD. An overall downward shift in measured intellectual ability was reported. In childhood, 46 % of the sample had scores in the severe range of intellectual disability compared to 71 % at follow-up.

Eaves and Ho (2008) followed 48 individuals with ASD from childhood to adulthood in Canada. Eight of the participants had a childhood IQ score above 70. All participants received special education support in childhood, and 30 % engaged in some kind of post-secondary educational program. Overall outcome adjustment ratings were that 21 % had Good or Very Good outcomes and 46 % had Poor outcomes. Almost 80 % received a government disability pension and used the services of social workers. Also in 2008, Cederlund et al. released their study of outcome for 70 adults with autism and 70 adults with Asperger’s disorder. Twenty-seven percent of this sample obtained an outcome categorization of Good, and only two people fell within the Poor category. There were no participants with Very Poor outcome ratings.

Our group (Farley et al., 2009) studied 41 adults who had been identified through a population-based study of ASD in Utah in the 1980s. All of these individuals had historical IQ scores of 70 or greater. Mean age at the first assessment was 7 years and in adulthood was 32 years. Outcome adjustment was better for this sample than previous samples. No systematically collected prognostic factors could be found to explain the difference, but anecdotal information suggested that the relative advantages experienced by the sample we studied over others could be related to the social supports experienced by most of the sample who were members of the Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-Day Saints (LDS Church) in Utah. LDS Church members tend to have large families and organize their religious communities according to the geographical location of their residences, so that in areas that contain a high density of LDS Church members children attend school and church activities with their neighbors. The members of individual congregations, therefore, tend to grow up having frequent interactions with the same community of individuals, with numerous inter-familial relationships due to family size. Participants in our sample routinely reported having found work, friendships, and roles in social groups through their relationships with other members of their church group.

A Canadian sample of 80 adults was identified through clinical diagnostic records, and outcomes were examined through chart reviews (Marriage et al., 2009). The investigators examined outcomes for 45 adults who were diagnosed before 18 years of age compared to 35 adults who were diagnosed in adulthood. As is often the case, individuals with comorbid intellectual disability experienced limited outcomes in adulthood. For those with normal range IQ scores, no differences were detected when comparing those diagnosed before 18 versus those diagnosed later. The investigators looked at a subgroup of individuals with normal range IQ scores who were over 25 years of age. Dividing this subgroup further based on whether they were diagnosed before or after age 18, they found that group diagnosed in adulthood had achieved more in terms of employment and independent living status. The authors suggested that the differences between these groups may be attributable to the fact that these subgroups had a 10-year age difference (average age of 29 for those diagnosed in childhood compared to an average age of 39 for those diagnosed as adults) that may have accounted for the relative increase in achievement levels for those diagnosed as adults. The authors speculated that perhaps having an additional 10 years to develop and achieve normative goals of adulthood offered an advantage to one group.

In a recent study of 20 US adults first examined under 4 years of age, Gillespie-Lynch et al. (2012) analyzed outcomes at an average age of 26 years. This was the fourth data collection point from this sample, with others occurring at average ages of 11 and 18 years. On average, participants had an average mental age of 2 years when they were almost 4 years old and 8 years when they were 18 years old. There was a trend toward reduction in ASD symptoms and improvement in adaptive functioning scores over time. Outcomes were rated as Very Good or Good for 30 % and Poor for 50 %. Early childhood language ratings and IQ scores predicted adaptive functioning in adulthood for this sample. A unique strength of this study was the nature of systematic data collection in early childhood that included specific metrics on the use of joint attention communication strategies. Initiation of joint attention, a voluntary communicative behavior, was not associated with adult variables, but response to joint attention, an involuntary communicative behavior, predicted adult social skills, ASD symptoms, and nonverbal communicative behavior.



Change in ASD Symptoms


Adults with ASD, as a group, exhibit reductions in the number and severity of ASD symptoms over time (Billstedt et al., 2005; McGovern & Sigman, 2005; Rumsey et al., 1985) although individuals may not exhibit notable improvements (Shattuck et al., 2007). In two investigations (Piven et al., 1996; Seltzer et al., 2003), researchers examined the durability of ASD symptoms using the ADI-R to collect detailed information on childhood and current symptom presentation. Almost all participants in both studies met full lifetime criteria for autistic disorder, but considerably fewer met criteria based on their current symptom presentation. Many of those who did not met the full criteria at the time of the investigations still exhibited impairments at or above the diagnostic threshold in two of the three behavioral domains. The authors noted that the ADI-R is designed to detect autism in young children and may not capture essential features in older people (Seltzer et al., 2003). Participants continued to experience clinically significant challenges in daily functioning as adults; therefore, childhood behaviors must be taken into account in diagnosis. Piven et al. (1996) emphasized the lifetime nature of these conditions in spite of notable symptom improvement.

Studies of social functioning in adults with ASD have illustrated lifelong, functional impairments for participants in social reciprocity and development of social relationships even though they exhibit substantial improvements in social abilities over time. Well-developed language skills were associated with better reciprocal social functioning in one study (Shattuck et al., 2007). McGovern and Sigman (2005) studied symptom changes in 48 late adolescents and early adults and reported large improvements on the ADI-R Social domain score. Participants’ lifetime mean scores were 12.93, with a current mean of 8.48. The authors observed significantly more improvement in the social abilities of individuals with IQ scores at or above 70. Howlin (2003) studied adult social functioning for a group of individuals with autistic disorder and average-range intellectual abilities. The group’s mean ADI-R Social domain score in adulthood was 11.09, surpassing the ADI-R threshold of 10 for social impairments suggestive of autistic disorder. Similarly, Piven et al.’s (1996) study of 38 adolescents and adults with autistic disorder and near-average or average range intellectual abilities yielded a current mean ADI-R Social domain score of 12.1. This score was a significant improvement over childhood mean score of 21.0 but still above the threshold for social characteristics of autistic disorder.

Like social abilities, communication skills typically improve by adulthood (Piven et al., 1996). Fifty-four percent of the adolescents examined in the outcome study by Rutter et al. (1967) exhibited improvements in communication skills. Significant improvements in verbal communication abilities have been reported on the ADI-R, although findings related to nonverbal communication have been mixed. McGovern and Sigman (2005) reported improvements in group scores for verbal individuals from a lifetime mean of 8.06 to a current mean of 3.56. Nonverbal individuals also demonstrated significant gains as a group, with a lifetime nonverbal communication mean score of 6.23 that diminished to a current mean of 3.82. These follow-up mean scores reflect communicative skills that no longer meet the ADI-R threshold for communication. Shattuck et al. (2007) reported on symptom change in 241 people aged 10–52, indentifying improvement in verbal communication but no improvement over a 4–5 year period for the sample in nonverbal communication skills.

Two outcome studies used information from assessments and caregiver interviews to code the overall language functioning of adults with ASD. Howlin et al. (2004) determined that 43 % of their sample demonstrated no or very little abnormality in adulthood in terms of speech functioning, and 43 % showed only mild abnormality. Use of language was more impaired, however, with only 41 % having no or mild impairments. Language ability was similar for the sample studied by Kobayashi et al. (1992), with 47 % giving evidence of no or mild impairments in the use of language.

Stereotyped or repetitive behaviors and restricted interests also persist into adulthood, according to all of the outcome studies in this review. Improvements are quite common, however. Perhaps because they are behavioral excesses, repetitive behaviors appear amenable to intervention targeted at cessation, at least in public. Rumsey et al. (1985) obtained detailed information on stereotyped or repetitive behaviors and restricted interests in 14 men through informant reports and 5-day observations on an inpatient unit. Additional data were collected through participant and caregiver interviews. A substantially higher proportion of restricted interests and repetitive behaviors were observed over the course of their stay than could be recorded in a single testing session. The authors reported that it seemed that some men, on recognizing that they were observed to engage in these behaviors, gave the impression of being ashamed to have been seen. Although this study reflects rates that are higher than is usually observed during brief assessments, caregiver reports indicated that these behaviors occur even more often at home than was recorded during the data collection period.

Results from studies using the Stereotyped Behaviors and Restricted Interests Domain of the ADI-R suggest that these behaviors appear equally in adult men and women with AD. Both genders demonstrate improvements in these areas by adulthood (Piven et al., 1996), and adults with IQ scores of 70 or better appear to improve much more than do those with lower IQ scores. McGovern and Sigman (2005) reported a mean decrease on this domain from 6.02 to 4.36. The outcome figure exceeds the ADI-R threshold of 3 and represents relatively less improvement in this area than in the social and communication domains in this sample. Howlin (2003) reported a similar domain score of 4.81 in her sample of adult males with autism.

A recent study of differences in restricted interests and repetitive behaviors with over 700 people with ASDs demonstrated that older individuals exhibited significantly fewer of these behaviors than younger people after controlling for gender, intellectual disability, and the use of psychotropic medications (Esbensen, Greenberg, Seltzer, & Aman, 2009). The sample consisted of roughly even groups of children, adolescents, and adults. About 60 % of the sample had comorbid intellectual disabilities, and 80 % were males. Adults with intellectual disability exhibited high levels of restricted interests and repetitive behaviors in comparison to younger people, while adults with normal-range IQ scores showed fewer of these behaviors than children did. Specifically, participants with intellectual disability had more severe stereotyped movements and more self-injury, but they did not exhibit higher levels of rituals, insistence on routine, compulsions, or restricted interests than other adults. For the entire sample, restricted interests decreased across age groups more than did other types of behavior within this domain.

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Nov 27, 2016 | Posted by in PSYCHOLOGY | Comments Off on Range of Outcomes and Challenges in Middle and Later Life

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access