Fig. 49.1
a A bidirectional model assumes that higher pain intensity prior to sleep onset will lead to greater disturbed sleep; and greater disturbed sleep will lead to increased pain. b A circular model assumes that pain disturbs sleep and subsequently poor sleep affects pain. Various factors likely impact both pain and sleep and the interaction such that neither a linear nor a circular model fully accounts for the complexity of pain-related sleep disturbance
Chronic pain patients are heterogeneous, and all pain is not the same. Chronic pain is associated with many comorbidities including anxiety, depression, specific sleep disorders, poor sleep hygiene, and symptoms associated with source of the pain, such as musculoskeletal disorders [1, 19], depression [20], or neuropathic pain [1]. These patients are also at risk for undergoing multiple surgeries or other types of invasive treatments. They often consume many types of prescription and over-the-counter medications with adverse and unpredictable effects upon sleep. Further, sleep quality and duration are influenced by physical activity, fatigue, daytime naps, and lifestyle behaviors such as smoking and the consumption of caffeine and alcoholic beverages. These factors have not been assessed or accounted for in the statistical analysis in many research studies. Finally, gender differences have been observed in sleep [21] and in the prevalence of clinical pain syndromes [22]; these differences have only begun to be studied in a systematic way in patients with chronic pain [23].
Does Pain Lead to Disturbed Sleep?
Pain is a unique, personal, and subjective unpleasant experience with a combination of sensory, affective, and behavioral dimensions, most often, associated with actual or potential tissue damage [24]. From studies of preclinical pain models, considerable evidence has accumulated about the plasticity and heightened sensitivity of pain transduction, transmission, and modulation processes in peripheral nerves [25], neurons, and synapses in the CNS [2] in response to various types of nociceptive stimuli. Evidence from clinical studies suggests that peripheral and central sensitization contributes to pain in arthritis, temporomandibular disorders, fibromyalgia, headache, complex regional pain syndrome, visceral pain, and post-surgical pain [2].
The models used to study pain processing have been categorized into three phases representative of the type of nociceptive stimulus and the extent of tissue injury [26], and is a useful framework to assess the impact of pain on sleep. Phase 1 pain involves the transient application of a nociceptive stimulus, which produces a minimal inflammatory reaction and brief vocal (cry, ouch), behavioral (reflex withdrawal, body movement), or physiological (altered neuronal activity) responses. Phase 2 pain occurs as a result of the application of substances that induce tissue damage and inflammation that persist for varying periods of time depending upon the agent used. Phase 2 pain is associated with release of mediators in tissues from damaged cells, blood vessels, and activated peripheral nociceptors. These chemical mediators directly activate or sensitize primary afferent nerve fibers to increase transmission of nociceptive information to the spinal cord invoking hyperactive neuronal responses that underlie the development of hyperalgesia (exaggerated response to nociceptive stimuli) and allodynia (pain-related response to non-nociceptive stimuli). Phase 3 pain involves damage specifically to neurons and nerve fibers. Behavioral signs of spontaneous and evoked pain responses persist for varying periods of time as do changes in the functional properties of CNS neurons and synapses. Peripheral and central sensitization is associated with a change in the gain of pathways involved in pain transduction, transmission, and modulation such that nociceptive stimuli or tissue injury are no longer necessary for pain to be perceived.
Phase 1 Pain Studies
Studies of phase 1 pain applied to animals or human volunteers during sleep yield evidence of transient sleep disturbance. In all of the studies, the experimenter [27] or subjects reported at least moderate-level pain to nociceptive stimuli (e.g., saline infusions into muscle, finger joint pressure, cutaneous laser heat) when they were awake [28–30]. In animal studies, behavioral withdrawal responses to the application of phase 1 laser heat pain were faster during slow-wave sleep (SWS) compared to waking. The animals fell back to sleep quickly when stimulated during sleep, and showed more prolonged pain-related responses (orienting to the site, licking the paw, changing body position) and took a longer time to return to sleep during waking compared to sleep [27]. These results were interpreted as evidence of a lower-pain threshold during SWS that were attributable to disinhibitory neuronal processes known to occur during sleep.
In studies with human volunteers, application of phase 1 pain stimuli leads to electroencephalographic (EEG) cortical arousal responses or brief awakenings during sleep [28–30]. It is difficult to compare results across studies because the type, duration, and intensity of nociceptive stimuli and protocols used in each study were quite different. Cutaneous heat of moderate [30] or high intensity [28] produced more frequent cortical arousals from non-REM (NREM) stage 2 compared to SWS and REM sleep, yet subjects reported the following morning minimal awareness of having been aroused or awakened during sleep. Nociceptive and non-nociceptive stimuli both produced a similar number of awakening responses from NREM stage 2 sleep and to reduced amounts of SWS and REM sleep obtained on experimental nights [31]. Findings from a laboratory study of sleep inertia (a transitional state of lower arousal) showed a large reduction in the perception of pain immediately after subjects were awakened from REM sleep, compared both to SWS and wake [32]. In a study of laser heat stimuli applied during sleep, investigators observed arousals to 31 % of the stimuli, and 53 % of these were associated with visual motor reactions on the polysomnogram. No reductions in the latency to the first component (N2) of the cortical EEG-evoked potential were observed, but the latency to the second component (P2) was shorter, and the evoked potential amplitude was reduced during both NREM stage 2 and REM sleep [33]. Evoked potentials measured over the frontal cortex were attenuated in REM sleep, although overall morphology of the response was similar to wakefulness. Non-nociceptive stimuli give rise to similar EEG-evoked potentials over the cortex such that specificity of nociception is inferred from the nature of the stimuli applied, not from the EEG response. Further studies are warranted to determine whether patients with chronic pain are more susceptible to nociceptive stimuli during sleep. However, responses to nociceptive and non-nociceptive stimuli in patients with chronic pain are quite different from that of healthy volunteers as brain activity could be distorted by changes already present in the CNS associated with the particular chronic pain condition [1]. Given these observations, comparisons between patients with different types of chronic pain may be more informative compared to healthy controls.
Phase 2 and 3 Pain Studies
Experimentally induced preclinical studies of phase 2 and 3 pain (systemic arthritis and sciatic nerve damage) show physiological changes in sleep architecture indicative of sleep disturbance and provide evidence that pain leads to disturbed sleep, but findings are not consistent for all types of pain models tested. Sleep in arthritic animals is highly fragmented [34, 35] with increased wakefulness during the inactive and increased sleep during the active periods of the diurnal cycle [36]. Sleep disturbance was most prominent during the acute inflammatory phase of arthritis [35–37] or during the acute postoperative period following nerve injury [38, 39]. A recent study in mice found a statistically significant small amount of increased wakefulness and reduced NREM sleep at 7 days and again at 28 days post-sciatic nerve injury [40]. Disease severity was associated with greater sleep disturbance and less wakefulness with increased delta activity during sleep in the active period in arthritic animals [36]. Although neuropathic pain models induce persistent peripheral nerve denervation along with long-lasting changes in gene expression in the CNS and behavioral signs of hyperalgesia and allodynia, no changes in EEG spectral power were observed in animals for over four months after injury [39]. Preclinical neuropathic pain models are produced by a variety of types of nerve injury that could lead to distinct differences in disease severity and differential impact on sleep and changes in EEG frequency patterns during sleep .
Findings from these studies of phase 2 and phase 3 pain provide evidence of disturbed sleep, but isolating pain, as the “cause” of disturbed sleep, is difficult to ascertain. Systemic arthritis evokes systemic inflammation, increases body temperature, and elicits a variety of “sickness behaviors.” Signs of disturbed sleep in peripheral nerve injury are most prominent in the acute postoperative period when systemic neuroendocrine responses to tissue injury, presumably, also are present. The most parsimonious explanation of these findings is an inability to sustain uninterrupted sleep episodes. Such changes could be related to persistent altered functioning of pain pathways, attributable to peripheral and central sensitization, that are more sensitive to slight bodily movements, or to environmental stimuli. Future studies of recently developed animals’ models of stress-induced generalized pain [41], and of combined inflammation and neuropathic injury [42], have potential to advance understanding of mechanisms involved in chronic pain effects on sleep.
Clinical Studies in Chronic Pain
Most studies of pain and sleep in clinical populations have been cross-sectional observational studies comparing self-reported pain and sleep disturbance in patients with different chronic pain conditions or controls without pain. Chronic pain has well-recognized disturbing effects on self-reports of sleep quality and disturbance, but subjects often are asked about how much pain interferes with sleep, or are given a list of factors or symptoms and asked to indicate which ones they attribute to disturbing their sleep [43]. Thus, studies may be biased toward finding false-positive reports of pain-related sleep disturbance. The majority of clinical studies that have used both subjective and objective measures of sleep have been conducted on patients with musculoskeletal conditions, and most of these involved patients with fibromyalgia. Chronic pain has not been consistently associated with disturbed sleep as measured by polysomnography (PSG) or actigraphy [44–47]. In these studies, often the self-report of poor sleep quality is out of proportion to modest changes in PSG indicators of sleep [9], especially when patients are compared to sedentary control subjects of similar age [45].
Sleep studies of patients with other types of chronic pain, in particular neuropathic pain, back pain, and chronic regional pain syndrome, are few in number. A recent review of the literature in patients with chronic low back pain found self-reported generalized sleep disturbance, reduced sleep duration, quality, and satisfaction, with increased sleep latency, and problems functioning in the daytime [48]. Of note, this evidence-based review included only two studies (meeting investigator inclusion criteria) that used PSG or actigraphy sleep measures, and only in < 10 patients in each study [49, 50]. A recent small clinical study of 16 patients with chronic low back pain yielded significant differences in self-reports of sleep disturbance compared to 16 control subjects, but no differences in sleep measures obtained from actigraphy [51]. It has been estimated that nearly 80 % of adults will, at some time, eventually experience back pain and metabolic biomarkers of brain function that correlate with clinical measures of chronic back pain [1]. We need PSG and nociceptive response studies during sleep in these clinical populations.
Investigators who preformed a structured evidence-based review of the literature (41 studies of good quality) on a variety of chronic pain conditions, that included some type of sleep measure, concluded that there is strong and consistent evidence to support an etiological role of pain with sleep disturbance due to a medical condition [7]. Of these, 77 % of 21 studies showed, through statistical modeling, that pain predicted the occurrence of a sleep problem, but only three of these studies were based on prospective analysis. Studies of daily temporal associations between pain and sleep with multilevel statistical modeling techniques have advanced our knowledge about pain–sleep interactions [9]. One of the first reports of a bidirectional relation between pain and sleep was derived from a prospective study in women with fibromyalgia [52]. Based on daily diary recordings over 30 days, a night of poor sleep was followed by more pain the next day; more pain during the day was followed by night of poorer sleep. Data on reports of nighttime sleep and pain for 8 days, from a large nationally representative sample of middle-aged adults in the USA, found that hours of sleep predicted subsequent frequency of pain and pain predicted subsequent hours of sleep, but the effect of sleep on pain was stronger than the effect of pain on sleep [53].
Recent studies of temporal associations between pain and sleep provide interesting and novel findings about the complexity of pain–sleep interactions. In a laboratory study of 75 children with juvenile arthritis, age, medications, anxiety, and evening pain explained 18 % of variance in subsequent EEG sleep arousals, but neither anxiety nor pain had a significant effect on sleep microstructure [54]. In a study of 97 adolescents with chronic pain or intermittent pain (healthy subjects), actigraphy-derived estimates of sleep duration and amount of wake after sleep onset, but not sleep efficiency or self-reported sleep quality, predicted next-day pain; daytime pain intensity did not predict subsequent nighttime sleep disturbance [12]. In a study of a group of 107 heterogeneous adult chronic pain patients (74 % women), both with moderate pain and insomnia severity scores, presleep mood and cognitive arousal, but not pain, predicted subsequent self-reported sleep quality; cognitive and somatic arousal predicted actigraphy-derived sleep efficiency [55]. Self-reported sleep quality and efficiency and actigraphy-derived sleep efficiency predicted next pain upon awakening, but predictors of pain in the first half or second half of the day varied. In post-hoc analysis, depression emerged as a significant predictor of self-reported sleep quality, along with presleep arousal. Thus, although relieving pain has potential to improve sleep, at the present time, it is not clear that pain is the most salient symptom to target in planning treatments to improve sleep in patients with chronic pain. Presleep arousal is an important factor influencing sleep quality and duration in insomnia [56], and therapies addressing arousal and mood [20] could be an important target for improving sleep in chronic pain patients.
Milestones: pain to sleep interactions
Advances in pain research have established that chronic pain is characterized by altered functioning in pain transduction, transmission, and modulation pathways in the brain leading to persistent heightened sensitivity to pain and the perception of pain after tissues have healed.
In animals, reflex withdrawal responses to pain stimuli are faster during NREM sleep and somewhat attenuated during REM sleep.
In humans, pain stimuli, applied during sleep, leads to transient arousals and sometimes brief awakenings but in healthy subjects awakenings are rarely remembered the next morning.
Substantial evidence exists to support pain in the etiology of self-reported, but not objective, measures of sleep disturbance in chronic pain.
Presleep arousal and mood, along with pain, have emerged as important factors associated with subsequent sleep disturbance.
Does Sleep Modulate Pain?
Sleep Effects on Pain
During sleep, an individual is unaware of environmental surroundings with reduced responsiveness to sensory stimuli. Arousal thresholds and response duration vary as a function of stimulus type and strength, sleep stage, time of night, age, and other factors. The literature on sleep modulation of sensory stimuli was reviewed previously and suggested that responses to nociceptive stimuli are preserved during NREM sleep, but gated to some extent during REM sleep [57]. In that review, reference was made to a series of studies of brainstem neurons in the region of the ventromedial reticular formation that previously had been implicated as one of the important sources for the mediation of descending nociceptive (pain) modulation or facilitation and hypothesized to inhibit nociception during sleep [58]. Research on brainstem neurons, OFF cells (anti-nociceptive) and ON cells (pro-nociceptive) have shown distinct patterns of spontaneous activity during waking, NREM, and REM sleep in rats [59] and more recently in mice [60]. OFF cells are continuously active during NREM sleep, sporadically active in wakefulness, and silent during REM sleep. ON cells are continuously active during waking, virtually silent during NREM sleep, and most active during REM sleep. Based on these response properties, one would expect that behavioral responses to noxious stimuli would be reduced during NREM sleep compared to wakefulness. However, paw withdrawal latencies to a radiant heat stimulus were faster during NREM sleep compared to wakefulness, and behavioral responses (e.g., licking the paw, moving about the cage) were suppressed during NREM sleep and exaggerated during waking [27]. In a recent study in mice [60], paw withdrawals to nociceptive heat were more reliably observed during sleep compared to wakefulness and associated with greater phasic activity of OFF cells prior to motor responses. Thus, rather than suppressing nociception , OFF cells appear to facilitate reflex motor protective withdrawal responses. However, how these neurons and others implicated in suppressing or facilitating pain responses would respond in the context of chronic pain has not been reported.
Sleep Loss Effects on Pain
Selective REM SD was one of the first types of sleep loss effects on pain studied. These experiments in rodents consistently have shown faster withdrawal latencies to noxious stimuli interpreted as reduced “pain” thresholds during [61], immediately after several days of deprivation [62–64], and after several days of recovery sleep [65]. Some evidence exists for reduced efficacy of opioid-mediated descending pain modulation in REM SD [66].
Increased pain perception or sensitivity has been observed in most experimental sleep loss studies in human volunteers, but such changes have not been consistently observed across studies involving the same type of nociceptive stimulus or similar SD protocols. One of the first total sleep deprivation (TSD) studies in human volunteers showed a progressive decrease in the threshold to pressure (von Frey hair), but not non-nociceptive stimuli [17]. One recent study has shown that the threshold to radiant heat pain was reduced after each of two nights of TSD compared to control subjects permitted usual sleep [67]; while the perception of reported pain on a visual analog scale was increased, the pain threshold was not altered after TSD in another study [68]. Selective SWS deprivation for three nights has been shown to increase muscle tenderness and reduce the threshold to pressure stimuli compared to baseline in young, primarily male, [69] and middle aged, female, subjects [70], but this change was not observed in another study [71]. Shorter latencies to radiant heat stimulus of 32 % have been observed after a single night of total sleep loss, after 4-h sleep loss, and after selective REM SD compared to non-REM deprivation [18]. Onen and colleagues [72] studied pain tolerance after one night of TSD followed by two consecutive nights of selective interruption of SW or REM sleep in counterbalanced order and a night of recovery sleep. Pressure, but not thermal, pain tolerance thresholds were reduced significantly after one night of TSD and remained lower after both SWS and REM sleep interruption nights, although the scores were not statistically different compared to baseline. After recovery sleep, there was a large increase in pressure pain tolerance scores, which was correlated with the amount of SWS during recovery. In one of the only reports of TSD in patients with chronic somatoform pain (e.g., without physical etiology) investigators reported increased pain perception, but without any change in pain thresholds to heat or pressure stimuli [73].
Compared to TSD, or selective sleep stage deprivation, inadequate or insufficient sleep is more common; one in three adults is estimated to sleep < 7 h per night [74]; thus, experimental studies of short sleep duration may provide data more applicable to sleep experienced in chronic pain conditions. In one of the few experimental studies of only women volunteers, neither three nights of interrupted sleep nor three nights of restricted, yet consolidated, sleep, both followed by a night of total sleep loss, led to any reliable group differences in pressure pain thresholds despite a 50 % reduction in total sleep time and increased sleepiness in both groups [75]. This study also assessed spontaneous pain symptoms and pain inhibitory processes using a test of diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC). In DNIC, the perception of a second nociceptive stimulus (e.g., pressure) is inhibited by the previous application of a nociceptive stimulus (cold) at a distant site. Only the group subjected to interrupted sleep showed an increase in spontaneous reports of pain symptoms and reduced pain inhibition. Another study involving assessment of endogenous pain-inhibitory processes after one night of experimental 4-h sleep restriction showed a 30 % increase in pain ratings, while reductions were observed in the amplitudes of laser-evoked EEG potentials; subjects were less able to inhibit pain when their attention was not focused on pain stimulus [76]. In a similar study of pain distraction, healthy individuals who habitually slept < 6.5 h, in the month preceding testing, had reduced pain inhibition, while playing video games, but they showed increased skin flare and augmented hyperalgesic responses to a heat-capsaicin stimulus compared to those who slept > 6.5 h [77]. In one of the only sleep restriction studies, found in the literature of patients with chronic pain, compared to control subjects, one night of 4-h sleep restriction augmented pain perception along with an increase in the number of painful arthritic joints, but fatigue, depression, and anxiety were also increased the next day [65]. With mild sleep loss, pain perception is augmented, both in healthy subjects and in patients with chronic pain, but increased pain perception is not always accompanied by evidence of altered pain thresholds. Such an increase in pain perception might be related to reduced functioning of pain-inhibitory processing.
Sleep Disorders and Pain
Sleep disorders often are associated with disturbed and fragmented sleep, and, in the case of insomnia, with short sleep duration, but few studies have measured pain perception or responses to nociceptive stimuli. Following the report of chronic pain by Ohayon [4] in a large percentage of people with insomnia, there is increased interest studying pain in primary and comorbid insomnia. Haack and colleagues [78] recently have shown that patients with primary insomnia reported twice as many days of spontaneous pain, showed reduced thresholds to heat and pressure pain, and both reduced pain facilitation and inhibition compared to control subjects. The results from this study were interpreted as evidence of subclinical pain and chronic activation of pain inhibitory mechanisms that produced a ceiling effect under challenging conditions. Additional research is necessary to confirm or refute this hypothesis.
Possible Mechanisms of Sleep Effects on Pain
Few studies have examined mechanisms that might underlie changes in pain perception with sleep loss [20]. Activation of inflammatory processes with increased circulating proinflammatory cytokines is one plausible mechanism that has been studied after sleep loss [79]. Increased C-reactive protein, an inflammatory biomarker of cardiovascular disease risk, was observed both after total sleep loss of 88 h or after 10 consecutive days of sleep restricted to 4 h nightly [80]. Increased plasma levels of interleukin-6, but not that of tumor necrosis factor alpha, have been observed following 10 days of 4-h sleep restriction [81], and increased urinary metabolites of prostaglandin E2 (PGE2) have been observed after 3 days of TSD [82]. In both studies, subjects reported spontaneous increased pain symptoms after several days of sleep loss. PGE2 is an important mediator of inflammation in peripheral tissues, capable of increasing peripheral nerve sensitivity [25] and neuron excitability in the spinal cord [83]. Proinflammatory cytokines released from circulating lymphocytes lead to release of cytokines by glia, in the spinal cord, and an increase in chemical mediators (e.g., excitatory amino acids, prostaglandins, nerve growth factors, and nitric oxide) that facilitate nociception and pain processing in the CNS [84]. Glia, rather than neurons, may be the biological basis of sickness behavior and therapies directed toward the upregulation of anti-inflammatory cytokines have been shown to reduce pain in animal studies [84].
Based on the results of the studies reviewed, it is not clear whether changes in pain perception and nociceptive thresholds with experimental sleep loss are attributable to total sleep loss, selective loss of a particular stage, restricting hours of sleep, or to sleepiness that inevitably accompanies sleep loss [85]. Sleepiness is one of the most reliable effects of experimental sleep loss. In fact, compared to well rested subjects, sleepy (as determined by multiple sleep latency tests) healthy subjects, after 8 h of sleep, showed increased pain sensitivity [86] and in a subsequent study, when subjects had extended time in bed, increased total sleep time correlated with reduced sleepiness and pain sensitivity [87]. As noted above, protective withdrawal reflexes to nociceptive stimuli are faster during NREM sleep and after experimental sleep loss when animals are “sleepiest.” Cytokines participate in the regulation of physiological sleep and are released after tissue injury, infection, and sleep loss [88]. Cytokines have a clear diurnal rhythm and reach the highest circulating levels during sleep [79]. It is possible that activation of cytokines during physiological sleep could participate in disinhibitory processes in the brainstem, which facilitate reflex withdrawal responses, yet protect sleep by not engaging sustained arousal responses? In addition to pain and sleepiness, cytokines have been associated with depressed mood, fatigue, impaired cognition, and memory—all symptoms that manifest with sleep loss. Thus, although activation of inflammatory processes is a plausible mechanism for increased pain sensitivity with sleep loss, further research is required to ascertain the role of cytokines and other inflammatory biomarkers to explain heightened pain following a night of disturbed sleep in patients with chronic pain, and in relation to levels of sleepiness.