, Marcy Willard1 and Helena Huckabee1
(1)
Emerge: Professionals in Autism, Behavior and Personal Growth, Glendale, CO, USA
Abstract
The autism phenotype includes social communication, inflexible language and behavior, and repetitive sensory and motor behavior. Our discussion will address these essential areas: assessment of communication, social interaction, play/imaginative use of materials, and restricted or repetitive behaviors. In discussing ASD assessment, it is important to carefully review the “Gold Standard” for observational assessment of autism spectrum disorders (Kanne, Randolph, & Farmer, 2008; Ozonoff, Goodlin-Jones, & Solomon, 2005) the ADOS, now in its second version (ADOS-2), and the ADI-R or diagnostic caregiver interview. Evaluation will be discussed across the domains of social communication and restricted repetitive behaviors, as well as sensory processing. This chapter provides examples of the assessments that might be used in all of these social and sensory areas. Assessment of core autism symptoms, explanation of the ADOS-2 items and algorithm, description of the ADI-R clinical interview, review of screening instruments, and review of sensory measures are included in this chapter. This chapter discusses sensory processing in the context of autism and other diagnoses instead of as a stand-alone diagnosis. These areas of assessment complete the core assessment of ASD while the following chapters include associated areas for comprehensive assessment.
Keywords
Social assessment of ASDSocial reciprocity in ASDADOS-2Assessing joint attention in ASDAssessing play behaviorsAssessing restricted interests in ASDRepetitive behavior in ASDSensory sensitivity in ASDSensory processing disorder? Is it real?Odd sensory behaviorsAssessing for Social and Core Autism Symptoms
Vignette #3 Alex: Autism Assessment in a Child with Social Interaction Challenges
Alex is 6 years old who was evaluated at Emerge for mood, social interaction, sensory, and behavioral challenges. He scored in the Superior range of intellectual ability on the WISC-IV and was reported to be bright, clever, and kind to others in class. Alex does not like it when his first grade classmates break rules and he wants to play the same pretend game “puppies” on the playground day after day. Alex has huge meltdowns at home over small things like disagreements with his sister. He is a bed wetter, seemingly unaware when he has to “go” and thus he often does not make it to the bathroom on time (Table 9.1).
Table 9.1
Assessing for social and core autism symptoms
Core area |
1. Social communication and core ASD symptoms |
• Overall score on ADOS algorithm (consider scores only valid if administered by qualified clinician) |
• Consider major symptom areas in light of entire evaluation |
• Review screening instruments: SCQ, GARS, CARS, M-CHAT, feedback from pediatrician |
• Behavior observations: appearance, behavioral presentation, language, eye contact and gesture use in communication, play/interests, social reciprocity, attention, motor skills, mood, and affect |
• Rating scales and interview on child’s social communication, behaviors, and overall functioning |
• Consider developmental milestones, social communication, and emotional regulation |
• Toddler: communication, reciprocal social interaction, restrictive and repetitive behavior |
• Module 1–3: social affect, communication, reciprocal social interaction; restricted and repetitive behavior |
• Module 4: communication, reciprocal social interaction, imagination/creativity, stereotyped behaviors, and restricted interests |
• Consider results from ADI-R, ADOS-2, AOSI, SCQ, SRS; clinical interview/observations |
Full measure names |
Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) |
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition, Module T-4 (ADOS-2) |
Autism Observation Scale for Infants (AOSI) |
Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) |
Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) |
Alex demonstrated an unusual style of communication and social interaction throughout the ADOS-2 and during other parts of testing. His speech was occasionally formal and advanced, and his utterances were sometimes characterized by an odd use of words or phrases. Though he occasionally offered information to the examiner during conversations, what he disclosed appeared to be blatant fabrications or events that were unlikely to be true. It seemed that Alex was unsure of how to respond to the examiner, and he dealt with this uncertainty by making up stories. For instance, he told stories about his dog catching ten squirrels at once, being on Fox News, and having a pet tiger. When asked follow-up questions about his pet tiger, he said the tiger is currently one inch long and is about to “turn two inches.” He seemed to lack any awareness of how the information he shared would be received by the examiner (i.e., that she would know he was being untruthful).
Alex occasionally asked the examiner about her ideas or experiences; however, he dropped several of the examiner’s conversational bids for him to ask questions and simply ignored her comments. Alex exhibited good eye contact and he directed some facial expressions appropriately at the examiner such as raising his eyebrows and smiling. Alex demonstrated extremely limited insight into typical social situations and relationships. For instance, he could not identify any of his friends’ names, and he shrugged when asked, “What makes someone a friend?” or “How is a friend different from someone whom you just go to school with?” Alex’s difficulty answering these questions is not commensurate with his strong verbal abilities, as evidenced by his performance on the WISC-IV.
Alex’s play was typified by unusual sensory interests and a highly specific focus on certain aspects of the play characters. For instance, when asked to play with several different objects, Alex first picked up a piece of cotton, held it in his hand, and said, “It’s a fluffy rock.” He also laid down the human figures, covered up their faces with plates, and said, “I don’t want to see their bald eyes.” Alex was diagnosed with ASD; and through the cognitive behavioral treatment that followed, Alex began to work on social perspective taking skills. He is becoming less bossy with his peers and his tantrums are reduced in frequency because his emotional awareness and coping skills have improved .
Assessing the Core Symptoms of Autism
Our discussion of core autism symptoms will address these essential areas: assessment of communication, social interaction, play/imaginative use of materials, and restricted repetitive behaviors. Georgiades et al. (2007) identified a factor structure for the autism phenotype that included social communication, inflexible language and behavior, and repetitive sensory and motor behavior. All of these factors associated with ASD are reviewed in this section. In discussing ASD assessment, it is important to carefully review the “Gold Standard” for observational assessment of autism spectrum disorders (Kanne, Randolph, & Farmer, 2008; Ozonoff, Goodlin-Jones, & Solomon, 2005) the ADOS, now in its second version (ADOS-2). The Autism Diagnostic Interview, Revised (ADI-R) parent or caregiver diagnostic interview is also discussed as a Gold Standard instrument. Evaluation will be discussed across the domains of social communication and restricted repetitive behaviors.
ADOS and ADI-R
Gold Standard Instruments
A review of the research indicates that the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS : Lord & Schopler, 1989) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R : Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994) are the tools generally recommended for autism evaluations and research (Tanguay, 2000). The ADOS-2 is an observation scale whereby a clinician assesses a child’s play and communicative behaviors through a naturalistic observation, using a defined set of activities and assessment criteria (approximately 14 activities, 26 items, and 3 domains). The ADI-R is a semi-structured interview, administered to parents (over 70 items, summarized into an algorithm that includes three domains/dimensions). Some studies name the ADOS as the “gold standard” (Klin et al., 2007) for diagnostic assessment of autism spectrum disorders. However, others refer to the ADI-R as the gold standard (Matson, Nebel-Shcwalm, & Matson, 2007). Matson and colleagues claim, “The ADI-R is considered to be the gold standard for assessment scales used in the diagnosis of autism by many at this time. It has a broader age range of norms, more published psychometric data, and is a best fit with DSM and ICD criteria relative to other scales…” (Matson et al., 2007, p. 43).
Finally, other studies suggest the use of both instruments as the gold standard. A comprehensive literature review conducted in 2013 by Falkmer, Anderson, Falkmer, and Horlin found that “…the true ‘gold standard’ classification and diagnosis of autism is still considered to be multi-disciplinary team clinical assessment , including use of the ADOS and ADI-R, as well as other assessments with consensus clinical judgment” (Falkmer, Anderson, Falkmer, & Horlin, 2013, published online no page number). Research on the usefulness of the ADOS and ADI-R in populations with co-occurring intellectual disabilities shows that these measures are valuable diagnostic tools (Sappok et al., 2013). Robertson and colleagues report that the ADOS and ADI-R measure slightly different aspects of manifestations of the disorder and the combination is a conservative approach to diagnose Autism Spectrum Disorders (Robertson, Tanguay, L’Ecuyer, Sims, & Waltrip, 1999). Another study focused on using these measures to assess preschool children and found good agreement between the two instruments (Dover & Le Couteur 2007). Research on the use of both instruments found approximately 75 % agreement between the ADI-R and ADOS (in 2006) (Dover & Le Couteur 2007). The authors of this text agree with the research claiming that the ADOS-2 and the ADI-R are both considered gold standard instruments, but argue that the purpose and usefulness of the instruments can be differentiated.
In looking at the utility of both the ADOS and ADI-R, both considered Gold Standard instruments, it is useful to consider sensitivity and specificity. For clarification, specificity measures the proportion of negatives (no diagnosis) correctly identified as such and sensitivity measures the proportion of actual positives (those with the diagnosis) correctly identified as such. Sappok and Diefenbacher’s findings indicate that the ADOS is a very sensitive measure with 100 % sensitivity but is lower in differentiating between diagnoses (45 % specificity) (Sappok et al., 2013). The study also utilized the ADI-R which had 88 % sensitivity and 80 % specificity. In the intellectually disabled population, it was more feasible to do the ADOS and less so to do the ADI-R, based on the number of caregivers available to be interviewed.
Studies have examined the relative practicality of utilizing both measures during an autism assessment. Both Filipek et al. (1999) and De Bildt et al. (2004) noted that the time to train on and administer both measures is extensive. In these authors’ experience, practicality is often discovered as the reason why some clinicians do not utilize both instruments consistently during diagnostic evaluations. Le Couteur et al. (2007) notes that for clinical and research settings the Autism Spectrum has broadened and a clear assessment framework is needed. As previously mentioned, the ADOS-2 offers observational data, whereas the ADI-R offers diagnostic interview information. Both interviews and observations have advantages and limitations . One advantage of using both is to “gather clinically relevant information in a systematic and comparable fashion.” These authors advance that the ADOS-2 is mandatory to include; however, most ADI-R data can be obtained through a comprehensive diagnostic interview meeting with parents. In more involved cases with diagnostic uncertainty, it is valuable to record behavior with both the ADOS-2 and ADI-R (Le Couteur et al., 2007).
History and Development of the ADOS-2 and ADI-R
According to the ADOS-2 manual, the first version of the ADOS was introduced in 1989 and intended for administration with children ages 5–12 with expressive language at least at a 3-year-old level. This measure was proposed to complement the Autism Diagnostic Interview, known as the ADI (Le Coutuer et al., 1989). The combination of the two measures was offered as having adequate reliability and validity for research diagnosis. For administration of the original ADOS, specific training was required. According to the manual, after the ADOS was released, it became evident that there was a need for a version of the measure appropriate for younger children and those with less language. The PL-ADOS was developed with this population in mind. At that time, the ADOS authors were participating in a longitudinal study that referred children at age two and further necessitated the development of this measure. The PL-ADOS, which came out in 1995, was more flexible and included more use of play materials (DiLavore, Lord, & Rutter, 1995). The ADI-R was released including more questions about social interaction and communication for younger children (Lord et al., 1994) making it a better companion measure.
The PL-ADOS was useful for assessing younger children with very limited speech but researchers found it still excluded two subsets of individuals. It did not identify young children with autism who also had some expressive language skills. It also failed to provide a method to assess and identify older adolescents and adults with autism. Test developers set to resolve these issues and the ADOS-G was developed. The ADOS-G is what we now know of as the ADOS. This version of the ADOS included Module 1 based on the PL-ADOS and Module 3 based on the original ADOS (1989). It included a “new” Module 2 developed for “phrase speakers” in the preschool age range and a Module 4 for assessing adolescents and adults. This module was less play based and included more questions about daily living, personal responsibility, and job skills. With the development of the ADOS-2, the original algorithms for Modules 1–3 were revised; a comparison score was created for Modules 1–3, and the ADOS protocols were improved to clarify administration guidelines. A fifth Module, the Toddler Module, was created with associated algorithms designed to assess children with limited expressive language who are aged from 12 to 30 months (Lord et al., 2012). For detailed psychometric data on reliability and validity of the measure see Appendix.
ADOS-2 Administration and Training Requirements
The ADOS-2 was originally released in May of 2012. It contains five assessment modules to evaluate varying developmental levels at different chronological ages. The modules assess skills in communication, reciprocal social interaction, and restricted and repetitive behaviors or interests. The ADOS-2 is a semi-structured measure including open-ended and creative tasks. The goal of the revision was to make the algorithm fit with continued research and provide increased specificity in lower functioning populations, as well as provide a uniform basis of comparison and maintain the strong predictive validity of the measure (Lord et al., 2012). Research findings indicated a need for a unitary social-communication factor (Constantino et al., 2004; Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 1999, 2000; Robertson, Tanguay, L’Ecuyer, Sims, & Waltrip, 1999). Modules 1–3 of the ADOS-2 include “social affect” or SA which is a composite of social and communication skills, and “restricted repetitive behavior” or RRB. The Module 4 algorithm was not revised because of limited available samples of older adolescents and adults, an area needing further study (Lord et al., 2012). Research on the ADOS Module 1 found it to be over-inclusive of those with a mental age of 15 months or below (Gotham et al., 2007) and thus the Toddler Module was developed for those 12–30 months old with play activities, codes, and an algorithm that is better aligned with this population.
In order to administer the ADOS-2, a clinician must be appropriately trained in assessment (i.e., have licensure and credentialing in psychology or medicine), have adequate practice with administration, and read the manuals thoroughly. Additionally, he or she must attend a workshop with an independent trainer or complete the video training program. It is crucial that a clinician has extensive experience in giving the measure and scoring the algorithm and this can take varying amounts of time depending on the clinician. In the authors’ clinical practice, administrators of the ADOS-2 should have completed the training and have co-scored the measure watching a reliable clinician and consistently achieving at least 70 % agreement on the algorithm overall. Additionally, it is required that trainees achieve the same diagnostic conclusion, with 100 % agreement, from the algorithm. This standard is consistent with what Dr. Lord’s team recommended for clinical practice at the ADOS training workshop (WPS, Ann Arbor, Michigan, June 2011). Those trained in the ADOS do not need an additional training for ADOS-2, only a thorough review of the manual, and completion of the ADOS-2 training video program for the Toddler Module. For research reliability, an individual must attend a research reliability workshop and achieve a greater interrater reliability than what is required for clinical work (WPS ADOS training workshop Ann Arbor Michigan, 2011).
ADOS-2 Administering and Readministering the Test
An important benefit of ADOS-2 administration is that, in addition to providing diagnostic information, it is excellent for guiding treatment planning. The ADOS-2 provides valuable information on pretend play, joint attention, flexibility, conversation and communication with others, restricted interests, unusual sensory interests, emotional awareness, and emotion regulation. Those with ASDs often have much more difficulty and sometimes present with resistance to ADOS-2 activities versus concrete cognitive or language testing. It is interesting to note whether a child preferred the ADOS-2 or other assessment measures. When introducing pretend play with toys, acting or demonstrating tasks, and conversations, often those who do not have ASDs are more excited or engaged with these measures than they are with cognitive measures.
In contrast, individuals with ASD may refuse to play with toys, citing their preference for video games, sports, books, etc. They often make excuses to avoid “acting” tasks; or they may say that they cannot do it or are too embarrassed. At other times, individuals with ASD might engage with the toys or books but have play skills that are not commensurate with cognitive and language abilities.
They may make up a story in a picture book but with no plot, character development or emotional response. In the authors’ experience, and consistent with past research, their responses on the storybook task alone can offer rich diagnostic data. Children with ASD often struggle to tell stories, fail to identify characters, struggle to describe the sequence of events, and cannot provide the main idea of the story (Suh et al., 2014; Willard, 2013) Some studies have found that children with autism actually perform similarly to neurotypical children when telling stories from picture books, but struggle with more challenging tasks requiring narrative recall (Losh & Gordon, 2014). Children with optimal outcomes may actually show improved narrative production over time, sometimes similar to neurotypical children. Thus, examiners should carefully consider the child’s narrative quality during the storybook task of the ADOS-2, particularly over repeated administrations.
Discussion of friendships and other relationships may be less sophisticated than expected or a child might often respond “I don’t know” or “I can’t remember.” Readministering the ADOS-2 periodically can provide nice data on progress, as goals for treatment may be improving pretend play skills, conversation or joint attention; the ADOS-2 can provide a measure of skill development over time. The authors recommend reevaluations every 1–2 years to provide progress data and direct treatment.
ADOS-2 Modules
Each module of the ADOS-2 provides the evaluator an opportunity to use a semi-structured approach to assess social communication and restricted, repetitive behaviors. The algorithm information provided in this section is from the ADOS-2 protocols and manual (Lord et al., 2012). Certain elements that fall under each domain are assessed and given a score that contributes to the overall ADOS-2 algorithm. Items are scored 0, 1, 2, or 3 based on what is observed during testing. A score of 0 indicates that the skill is present and consistent with typical development. A score of 1 indicates partial proficiency on a skill. A score of 2 or 3 indicates challenges with the item or an absence of the skill. Some items may be scored 7 or 8 indicating another challenge with language or behavior, for example. A score of 3 is recalculated as a 2 in the algorithm and 7 or 8 are scored as 0. The ADOS-2 takes approximately an hour to administer and should be scored within an hour after administration. Not all elements scored fall into the algorithm. These items not included in the algorithm vary to some degree based on the module administered.
The Toddler Module is administered to those falling from 12 to 30 months old. Toddlers can have “few to no words” or “some words” and items fall into the algorithm depending on the level of language. The Toddler Module focuses on responses to interaction from parents/caregivers and evaluators. Module 1 also provides a different scoring algorithm for those with limited words and with “few to no words.” Module 2 is administered to those with phrase speech and Module 3 is for individuals with an expressive language level of 4 years old or above. Module 3 is administered to those through childhood and into adolescence while Module 4 is intended for those who are teenagers, adults, and have more independence. Module 4 is less play based and more focused on conversation, responsibility, and emotional communication. In the section that follows, readers find a discussion of each module and the assessment of social interaction/communication and restricted/repetitive behaviors.
ADOS-2 T
Communication
Frequency of Spontaneous Vocalization Directed to Others
This item is scored for children with few to no words and focuses on the amount of spontaneous vocalization. This vocal production may include sounds and partial words that are directed to another person for the purpose of engagement. For example, a child may say “da!” and clearly direct that communication to another person as a form of communication (smile, point, touching another). This type of utterance counts as a vocalization. However, any sounds that are directed to a toy, other object, or to the child himself or herself (back turned to adults, no eye contact or attempt to involve a person) would not be included as a directed vocalization in an examiner’s assessment of this skill.
Pointing
This item is scored for young children who do use language and refers to whether the child points distally to an object, toy, person, snack, or other object to reference it when it is out of reach. Coordinated pointing indicates a score of 0. Pointing only to an object within reach or using an open-handed grab is not considered coordinated, and may be assessed here as a 1 or 2.
Gestures
Gestures can be informational, descriptive, instrumental, or conventional and refer to whether a toddler communicated nonverbally during the assessment. The authors’ experience is that many clinicians struggle with understanding the subtle differences in type of gesture. As a clear understanding is critical to scoring, clinicians are advised to review the ADOS-2 manual in depth and consult with trainers and expert administrators to clarify nuances in gestures. In short, descriptive gestures include “the giraffe was this big!” (with an obvious wide reaching hand motion) and “the ant was tiny” (with a small reach or coordinated finger gesture to indicate small). Informational gestures may indicate how many of something (a show of fingers). Instrumental may include shrugging, nodding, shaking the head, waving, putting up a hand for stop, pointing or shushing (Attwood, Frith, & Hamelin, 1988), and conventional refers to clapping for “well done,” showing, and pointing (Kientz, Goodwin, Hayes, & Abowd, 2013). Gestures must contribute to the understanding of a communicative intent. Gestures are assessed in play and in engagement with parents and evaluators. Nodding or shaking one’s head no is a gesture, though it is not as advanced as descriptive gestures used to explain or clarify something for the listener. Spontaneous use of gesture receives more credit than gestures that are prompted and occur in response to a task.
Reciprocal Social Interaction
Unusual Eye Contact
The eye contact item refers to eye gaze that is too intense or the avoiding of eye gaze. A child who is constantly looking down or to the side is generally not making eye contact with the evaluator. Some may avoid eye contact because of shyness so the extent to which this improves over the course of testing is considered. Eye contact that is consistently poor or inconsistent, with the child looking away or looking at the examiner sometimes while avoiding eye gaze completely at other times is considered unusual, and should be scored a 2. Clinically, most examiners note that if there is any sense that eye contact was awkward or not integrated to facilitate communication, then this item should automatically be coded a 2.
Facial Expression Directed to Others
Facial expressions are important considerations in autism. The examiner must consider if the facial expressions are typical and congruent with the conversational context. They must further consider whether or not the client’s facial expressions are directed at the examiner. A facial expression that is accompanied by vocalization and eye contact certainly would qualify as directed to someone. Generally speaking, any facial expression that is directed, even a puzzled look toward the examiner when “blocked” toy play is going on, would be a directed facial expression. An individual who maintains a flat affect, whose expression is unusual or inappropriate, or who does not direct facial expressions to another person would receive a score of 2.
Integration of Gaze and Other Behaviors During Social Overtures
The examiner is now required to code the integration of gaze and gestures with any social overtures that occur during administration of the ADOS-2 T. When young children integrate their gaze, speech, and facial expression, this item would be coded 0. Toddlers who use these communication modalities separately (not in an integrated fashion) or show less consistency with the integration of them would receive a 1 or a 2.
Shared Enjoyment in Interaction
Throughout the ADOS-2, skilled examiners are constantly assessing the quality of the interaction. Individuals who are reciprocal and seem to share enjoyment with the examiner likely do not have autism. When administering toy-play items on the ADOS-2 T, it is extremely important to pay attention to whether the child is sharing enjoyment in the interaction or only likes the toy. Some children really enjoy play and will squeal delightfully while playing with a pop-up toy, play characters, or letter blocks. This is a lovely play skill but does not indicate shared enjoyment. The same child may continue to face his or her back toward the examiner, try to use the examiner’s hand as a tool, or take the toy away from the examiner. This is a particularly crucial item to attend to during the ADOS-2 T. It can be very helpful to watch how hard the parents are used to working in order to gain their child’s attention in general.
Response to Name
Response to name involves calling a child’s name up to four times when he or she is engaged in something else, though not overly engaged. If there is no response, the parents or caregivers are asked to call the child’s name twice. If this does not work, parents are asked to indicate how they might get their child’s attention without touching him or her. If none of these are effective, it can be helpful to see how a parent engages their child even if this may be by tickling, swinging them around, or hugging them.
Ignore
The “ignore condition” refers to a situation where the behavior of a child is evaluated while when he or she is ignored or blocked from accessing a toy by the examiner. It is important to note whether the child cries or cares at all about being ignored and the general kind of response that is present. A child may not respond at all, simply moving onto something else or he or she may try to move the examiner like an object. If a child makes eye contact or says something like “hey!” or “no!” then, he or she would receive a score of 0. Those who do not respond to the ignore condition receive a higher score.
Requesting
One potential impairment in autism is making requests . The term “manding” at it applies to autism, refers to verbal requests in order to obtain reinforcement. On the ADOS-2 T, young children may request a toy, snack, interaction, or other involvement from the parent or caregiver. A child can request with a gesture, words, or eye contact. Note, only partial credit is given for requesting only during the snack time.
Showing
The “showing” item refers to whether or not a child shows a toy, snack, anything of interest to a parent or an evaluator. Showing involves holding something up where another person can see it, not for the purpose of getting help, but only for the purpose of engaging the other person.
Spontaneous Initiation of Joint Attention
Spontaneous initiation of joint attention means that the individual being evaluated attempts to gain the attention or engagement of the examiner. Initiating joint attention is less frequently seen in young toddlers and so response to joint attention is often of more interest in this module. Initiation of joint attention requires the child say something like “look” or point and reference something for the parent or caregiver in order to get that person’s attention. A multiple point initiation would then involve looking at the target of interest, and then looking back at the caregiver or examiner.
Response to Joint Attention
This involves responding to an evaluator’s attempt to get attention just with his or her eyes. A child may see an evaluator looking at something and follow his or her gaze to check out what is being looked at. In this case, a toy bunny is placed across the room in triangular position to the child and examiner without the child seeing the object before the examiner calls attention to it. If the child sees the bunny first, the item is spoiled. The examiner has four chances to say “Johnny, look” or “Johnny look at that!” while only drawing attention with the eyes and face. If the evaluator has to point to the object of interest to draw attention, then the item only receives partial credit. A child who looks at something the examiner references with “Hey (child) look!” and then looks back at the examiner to laugh or share a reaction is doing this very well and receives a 0. If an individual ignores the examiner, or only looks once the toy is activated, he or she is scored 2 or 3.
Quality of Social Overtures
Quality of social overtures is an assessment of the quality of the efforts made by the child to get someone’s attention. Initiating to get something like a snack is less valuable than initiating to share toys, play a game, or give a hug.
Amount of Social Overtures Parent/Caregiver
This refers to the amount of social interaction indicated toward the parent or caregiver who is in the room. It is very helpful to see how a child interacts with his or her parents and how hard a parent must work to scaffold a social interaction with the child. Some children talk reciprocally with their parents (0) and others ignore their parents entirely (2).