Assessment and Treatment of Deficits in Social Skills Functioning and Social Anxiety in Children Engaging in School Refusal Behaviors

 

0 days

1–2 days

3–4 days

5+ days

4th

8th

4th

8th

4th

8th

4th

8th

1994

52

44

30

33

11

13

7

9

1998

53

44

30

34

11

14

6

8

2002

52

45

30

35

11

13

6

7

2003

49

44

30

35

13

14

8

8

2005

52

45

29

35

12

13

7

7




Table 2.2
Percentage of students, by grade, with English-language accommodations or other school support











































































































































































 
1994

1998

2002

2003

2005

4th

8th

4th

8th

4th

8th

4th

8th

4th

8th

Total

18

22

17

22

18

20

22

22

19

20

ELL

 Yes



23

26

20

23

20

23

21

23

 No



17

22

18

20

22

22

19

20

Classified as having a disability
                   

 Yes



26

31

23

28

27

30

24

29

 No



16

21

17

19

21

21

19

20

Free Reduced Lunch

 Eligible



21

26

21

24

25

26

23

25

 Not Eligible



14

20

16

18

20

19

17

18

Location

 Central City

20

24

17

22

18

21

22

23

20

22

 Urban fringe/large town

17

21

16

21

17

20

20

20

18

20

 Rural/small town

17

20

18

23

18

19

23

22

20

19


ELL  =  English Language Learner




History of Classification Systems


Given the variety of reasons a student may be absent from school, a number of theories regarding classification systems have been developed to describe the phenomena that lead a student to engage in behaviors such as refusing or attempting to refuse school or to experience great distress when at school. When researchers began to see chronic school absenteeism as a clinical concern, rather than merely a more common feature of delinquency as was typically described (e.g., Healy, 1915; Burt, 1925; Williams, 1927), the initial descriptions of nonattendance were ­primarily related to the role of anxiety in chronic school absenteeism.

Broadwin (1932) described two types of “truants;” first those who were truant for more traditional reasons such as, “a loss of interest because of inability to keep up with the pace of the class or because the child can do more advanced work, unwitting and even willful encouragement of the parents, and ‘bad’ associates,” (p. 253) and secondly, those students who are truant because of, “a deep seated neurosis of the obsessional type or displays a neurotic character of the obsessional type” (p. 254). Broadwin (1932) suggests that these children are in need of additional study and describes them as students who are “miserable, fearful, and (will) at the first opportunity run home despite the certainty of corporal punishment”. This description of truancy as a function not of an aversive environment or competing reinforcement outside of school but of a neurotic character led to additional work looking at school absenteeism as a clinical problem rather than a delinquency one.

Partridge (1939) described five types of groups engaged in truancy: an undisciplined group, a hysterical group, a desiderative group, a rebellious group, and a psychoneurotic group that was markedly different from the first four groups. Similar to Broadwin’s second group of truants, Partridge described the psychoneurotic group as individuals whose behavior was not simply a means of escaping environmental concerns or fulfilling wants but instead reflected an overabundance of anxiety. Partridge also noted that this group frequently had an overprotective parent.

Johnson, Falstein, Szurek, and Svendsen (1941) spoke similarly about an emotional disturbance that led to prolonged absences from school, which they referred to as “school phobia.” Similar to Partridge and Broadwin, Johnson et al. reported a subset of school refusers for whom anxiety was considerable, which were different from those who were seen as simple truants. Johnson et al. suggested that school phobic children had an acute anxiety that was caused by either an emotional conflict or an organic disease. The children’s anxiety subsequently created an increase of anxiety in their mothers, which was followed by a poorly resolved dependent relationship of these children to their mothers.

Building on the “school phobia” diagnosis, Coolidge, Hahn, and Peck (1957) talked about school absenteeism as something specific to the school and not wholly related to the dependent nature of children’s relationships with their mothers. Like the Johnson description, Coolidge et al. described a neurotic type of school phobia that was characterized by younger children with anxiety symptoms that suddenly occurred. Unlike the Johnson descriptions, Coolidge et al. also included a more traditional group of school refusers who were typically older and had a more gradual onset of school refusal behaviors. This group was similar to the non-anxiety groups described by Broadwin (1932) and Partridge (1939) while still adhering to the school phobia term.

Kennedy (1965) continued on the Coolidge et al. (1957) dichotomy related to school phobia. He described school phobia as being either Type 1, having acute onset, or Type 2, reflecting a “way of life” that was more gradual in development and more chronic in nature. He suggested that both types had common symptoms including:

(a)

Morbid fears associated with school attendance and a vague dread of disaster

 

(b)

Frequent somatic complaints: headaches, nausea, drowsiness

 

(c)

Symbiotic relationship with mother, fear of separation

 

(d)

Anxiety about many things: darkness, crowds, noises

 

(e)

Conflict between parents and the school administration

 

Despite their similarities, Kennedy maintained that the two types were two different categories of disorders that would require differing types of treatments.

Berg, Nichols, and Pritchard (1969) continued classifying school phobic children as acute (nonproblematic school attendance for at least 3 years prior to the current episode) and chronic (all other cases) but added additional classification requirements:

1.

Severe difficulty in attending school, often amounting to prolonged absence.

 

2.

Severe emotional upset, shown by such symptoms as excessive fearfulness, undue tempers, misery, or complaints of feeling ill without obvious organic cause on being faced with the prospect of going to school.

 

3.

Staying at home with the knowledge of the parents when they should be at school, at some stage in the course of the disorder.

 

4.

Absence of significant antisocial disorders such as stealing, lying, wandering, destructiveness, and sexual misbehavior (p. 123).

 


Definitional Issues


While there are a number of similarities across each of these explanations of excessive absence, and though they frequently use similar terminology (with varying degrees of relatedness) there are a number of differences as well. Differences across the foundation of and use of classification systems have made it difficult for researchers and clinicians to come to a consensus about the definition and classification of students who engage in school refusal behaviors. This difficulty is bolstered by the fact that both the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and proposed DSM-V diagnostic categories (American Psychiatric Association, 2011) do not include a specific formal diagnosis related to problematic absenteeism. Instead, school refusal behaviors are typically addressed under coexisting conditions that often occur comorbidly with school refusal behaviors. These can include but are not limited to Oppositional Defiant Disorder, Conduct Disorder, Separation Anxiety Disorder, Panic Disorder with Agoraphobia, Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Social Anxiety Disorder, and Specific Phobia. While all of these diagnoses could be related to school refusal behaviors, it does not necessarily follow that a student who engages in school refusal behaviors would qualify for any of these disorders.

Despite the long standing theoretical bases that have led to these differing classification systems, they all leave something to be desired because of the number of different environmental contingencies that lead to the same behavior, school refusal. This is easily seen in the number of differing nosologies related to the same behaviors. Whether considered school phobia or psychoneurotic truancy, the behaviors being described are similar and could be related to a number of common symptoms as suggested by Kennedy (1965). Even though distinctions such as chronic vs. acute and anxiety related vs. conduct disordered may be useful in classification, it does not stand to reason that a student who is engaging in school refusal for the first time (acute) has not been anxious about school for a long period of time. Additionally, problematic conduct outside of school does not necessarily mean that a student does not have debilitating anxiety problems within school. Students who engage in externalizing problematic behaviors are not necessarily free from internalizing problems or social anxiety. Research has repeatedly found that individuals referred with school refusal problems have been comprised of a number of subgroups including individuals with anxiety disorders, depressive disorders, and both (Bernstein, 1991; Bernstein & Garfinkel, 1986).

This heterogeneity of school refusers led Kearney and Silverman (1993) to create a functional model of child-motivated school refusal behaviors. In this model they aimed to examine school refusal behaviors from a functional point of view, probing environmental contingencies that could reinforce school refusal behavior, rather than only assessing perceived diagnostic correlates that use internal states to explain behavior. This functional view allows a greater direct link from behavioral function to treatment. Similar to prior functional explanations of behavior (e.g., Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & Richam, 1994; Durand & Crimmins, 1988), Kearney and Silverman break maintaining variables broadly into positive and negative reinforcement and then more specifically into avoidance of stimuli providing negative affectivity, escape from aversive social or evaluative situation, attention getting behavior, and positive tangible reinforcement.

Given the number of differing definitions of school refusal behaviors suggested over the years and taking into consideration data regarding differing functions related to topographically similar behaviors, the authors of this paper would like to endorse the use of the Kearney and Silverman definition of school refusal behaviors as a means to describe this class of behaviors. Kearney and Silverman (1996) describes school refusal behavior as, “child-motivated refusal to attend school or difficulties remaining in classes for an entire day.” They go on to say:

this definition includes youth aged 5–17 years who, to a substantial extent, (a) are completely absent from school, and/or (b) initially attend then leave school during school days, and/or (c) go to school following behavior problems such as morning temper tantrums, and/or (d) display unusual distress during school days that precipitates please for future nonattendance. (Kearney & Silverman, 1996, pp. 345)

This definition encompasses a number of historical classifications including delinquent truancy, school phobia, and anxiety-based absenteeism. While research on functional profiles of students engaged in school refusal behaviors shows that many profiles do at times match prior definitions (i.e., that students motivated by negative reinforcement were more often reporting high levels of fear and anxiety than those in positive reinforcement groups (Kearney, 2002; Kearney & Albano, 2004) this model allows students who are engaging in school refusal behaviors for multiple reasons (mixed functions) to be included under one umbrella definition.


Anxiety Related School Refusal


A study by Weeks, Coplan, and Kingsbury (2009) investigated both what correlates with social anxiety in childhood and what the consequences may be for children who experience symptoms of social anxiety. Their sample included 178 children in second grade. They found that anxious students liked school less and avoided school more than their non-anxious counterparts. They also found that anxious students reported themselves as more lonely at school than same aged non-anxious students. Additionally, anxious students’ teachers perceived them as weaker students academically than the non-anxious students. These findings suggest that anxious students who dislike school are likely to display more school refusal behavior than non-anxious students.


Assessment Tools


Because of the great heterogeneity related to school refusal behaviors and myriad of theoretical explanations for these behaviors, a number of assessment procedures have been utilized over the years to assess school refusal. As a means of covering multiple sources of assessment procedures, the current authors chose to report on a variety of assessments used to examine school refusal. These may be of varying benefit depending on the nature of school refusal. It is suggested that multiple methods are used when examining behavior, but that in all cases, assessments be used to inform intervention.


Diagnostic Interviews



Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Child and Parent Version (Silverman & Albano, 1996)


The ADIS is a semi-structured diagnostic interview that can be used to assess school refusal and related problems in youth ages 6–18 (Silverman & Albano, 1996). The Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Child and Parent Version (ADIS for DSM-IV:C/P) has both a child and a parent interview with questions in regard to school refusal behaviors that have occurred within the last year (King & Bernstein, 2001). The interview consists of six yes/no questions in relation to school refusal, including items, such as, “do you get very nervous or scared about having to go to school?” and, “do you miss or leave school early because you like it better home?.” There are additional open-ended questions aimed at uncovering why school is anxiety-provoking and determining the duration of the school refusing behavior. The final part of the school refusal section includes 15 items common in a school setting (such as speaking to other people and taking tests) that are rated on a 0–8 scale for degree of fear for that item and for how much fear of that item interferes with the ability to attend school (Silverman & Albano, 1996). Silverman and Albano (1996) emphasize that significant scores on the school refusal behaviors section on the ADIS for DSM-IV:C/P require follow-up within DSM-IV diagnostic categories to better understand the nature of the problem.

In addition to the section on school refusal behaviors, the ADIS for DSM-IV:C/P includes sections for the assessment of each of the nine diagnostic categories of anxiety listed in the DSM-IV, sections for the diagnosis of mood disorders, and a section for the identification of externalizing disorders (Silverman & Albano, 1996). This large range of categories makes the ADIS for DSM-IV:C/P a useful tool to help determine the nature of school refusal behaviors and identify possible comorbid disorders (King & Bernstein, 2001; Silverman & Albano, 1996). The ADIS for DSM-IV:C/P has been shown to have good inter-rater reliability and test–retest reliability (for combined child and parent interviews: κ  =  0.84 for separation anxiety disorder, κ  =  0.92 for social phobia, κ  =  0.81 for specific phobia, and κ  =  0.80 for generalized anxiety disorder; Silverman & Ollendick, 2005). Additionally, it has been shown to have concurrent validity with the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC; March, Parker, Sullivan, Stallings, & Conners, 1997; Silverman & Ollendick, 2005).

An example of use of the ADIS for DSM-IV:C/P in youth with school refusal behavior is a study by Kearney and Albano (2004), in which they used the interview to obtain DSM-IV diagnoses for 143 school-refusing children, aged 5–17 years. Of that sample, close to a third did not meet criteria for a DSM-IV diagnosis with the remaining two-thirds meeting diagnostic criteria for primarily anxiety disorders, mood disorders, or conduct disorders.

To further assess for the presence of anxiety disorders in youth with school refusal, it can be advantageous to utilize self-report measures (King & Bernstein, 2001).


Survey and Self-report



Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale-Second Edition (Reynolds & Richmond, 2008)


The Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale Second Edition (RCMAS-2) is an updated version of the Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (Reynolds & Richmond, 1985), the most common self-report measure for anxiety disorders in children (Silverman & Ollendick, 2005). It was normed with an ethically diverse sample of more than 2,300 children between 6 and 19 years, with separate norms for three age groups 6–8 years, 9–14 years, and 15–19 years. The RCMAS-2 consists of 49 yes/no items, intended to cover physiological anxiety, worry, social anxiety, and defensiveness. In addition to these scales the RCMAS-2 has a new cluster of items meant to assess performance anxiety. The RCMAS, which scales correlate highly with the RCMAS-2 had an internal consistency of above 0.80 and test–retest reliability ranging from 64 to 76 across total scale and subscales (Reynolds & Richmond, 1985; Silverman & Ollendick, 2005).

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Mar 22, 2017 | Posted by in PSYCHOLOGY | Comments Off on Assessment and Treatment of Deficits in Social Skills Functioning and Social Anxiety in Children Engaging in School Refusal Behaviors

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access