Mechanisms Involved in Memory Processes: Alterations Induced by Psychostimulants—Targeting the Central AT1 Receptors

A419250_1_En_14_Tab1b_HTML.gif



The psychostimulant administration was observed to either improve (+), interfere (−) or have no effect (=) in the performance of rodents and humans in several learning trials: PA passive avoidance, AA active avoidance, PCA Pavlovian conditioned approach, FC fear conditioning, RS retrieval of emotional and unemotional stimuli, APST affective picture system test, PAL paired-associate learning, LI latent inhibition, FR free recall, CDS conditioned drink suppression, MT maze task



A419250_1_En_14_Fig1_HTML.gif


Fig. 14.1
Interfering effect of Amph over long-term memory consolidation. (a) Schematic representation of experimental protocol performed to evaluate Amph effect over consolidation in the one-trial step-through inhibitory avoidance test. (b) Amph effects on consolidation of a fear memory were observed as interference when administered after training with a medium shock-intensity protocol (0.5 mA). * different from saline injected-animals, p < 0.05


From what it is reported, Amph appears to have a facilitatory role on retrieval stage in various animal models of memory [52, 54, 55]. In passive avoidance test, Kovacs et al. noted an improvement in performance in rats which had been treated 1 h previously with Amph [54]. Similar results were reported by Stone et al. in mice. However, these results have not been reproduced in humans, given that no significant differences were found upon administration of Amph in various memory tests [40, 41, 56].



The Brain Renin–Angiotensin System


The classical effects regarded for the Renin–Angiotensin System (RAS) were related to its endocrine role in electrolytic homeostasis and control of blood pressure. Angiotensin II (AngII) control implies a rapid increase in vascular resistance, and long-term effects acting on vasculature, heart, kidney, sympathetic output, and the central nervous system (CNS), by promoting vasopressin and aldosterone release and regulating thirst and water intake [6063].

Among the receptors that have been identified to be activated by AngII, the AT1 receptor (AT1-R) is the one mediating most of the peptide’s physiological and pathological functions. This surface receptor belongs to the G-protein-coupled receptor family, and has a conformation of seven transmembrane domains [6365]. AT1-R activates multiple intracellular signaling pathways; mainly, it promotes IP3 formation and Ca2+ release from intracellular compartments, adenylate cyclase inhibition, modulation of voltage-dependent Ca2+ channels and activation of PLC. Secondary pathways involving MAPK, ERK or JNK activation have also been described for its participation in trophy events. Independent G-protein activation pathways involve later desensitization and even internalization by endocytosis mediated by β-arrestins [63].

Over the years, it has come to be appreciated that a local autocrine or paracrine RAS exist in a number of tissues, which subsequently implies new roles for this system [66]. Moreover, tissues and systemic activities show significant differences. Even though circulating AngII levels may not be intensively high, AT1-R expression in different organs can be abundant enough to promote intracellular signaling. Furthermore, locally produced AngII concentration may be higher than plasma levels, and elicit a response in tissues with relative low AT1-R expression.

Numerous pieces of evidence support that AngII acts as a neuromodulator in different brain pathways. The activation of the AT1-R subtype by AngII elicits neuronal depolarization through facilitatory activity over different channels. The AT1-R facilitator effects would be possible through the inhibition of potassium channel or opening of a non-selective sodium–calcium channel. The resulting depolarization triggered by the neuropeptide over different signaling pathways would reflect a fine regulation in overall cellular activation [6769].

Across the CNS, AngII plays an important role in central dopaminergic neurotransmission modulation, and there is evidence that a cross-regulation exists between these two systems. It has been demonstrated that AT1-R modifies tyrosine–hydroxylase activity and hence alters monoamine production. DA release is elevated in a concentration-dependent manner when AngII is applied either to striatal slices or locally in the rat striatum. Furthermore, microdialisys of AngII in the same brain structure increases the level of DA and its metabolites (DOPAC and HVA). Similar effects have been obtained for DA concentration in NAc after AngII administration [7072]. AT1-R blockers did not only reverse these changes in the neurotransmitter levels but it also decreased its concentration without AngII stimulus, suggesting a regulatory role on basal DA release [72, 73]. All together, these results point towards the possible action of AngII pre-synaptically modulating the synthesis and release of DA through activation of AT1-R, as they are located in the soma and terminals of dopaminergic neurons [74]. Moreover, recently it has been shown that an alteration in the dopaminergic system, such as decreased levels of DA, can induce an increase in local RAS components, as an attempt to compensate for the deficit in dopaminergic activity [74]. Therefore, AT1-R expression is closely linked to DA levels, given that decreased levels of DA promotes an increase in AT1-R expression, which can be reversed when the DA activity is reestablished. Similarly, the knockout mice for D1 and D2 receptors, have increased levels of AT1-R, while transgenic mice overexpressing D2 receptors show reduced levels [75, 76]. Moreover, aged rats show decreased levels of DA receptors and increased expression of AT1-R simultaneously, when compared to young animals [77]. In the same way, acute or chronic manipulation of brain RAS with AT1-R antagonists decreased D2 and increased D1 receptor expression without affecting striatal DA release or motor behavior [78]. Recent evidence highlights functional and physical interactions between AT1-R and β- adrenergic, D1 and D2 receptors [7981]. Blockade of the D2/AT1-R heterodimer by an AT1-R antagonist, leads to enhanced dopaminergic transmission and has direct impact on the basal ganglia system involved in motor control. Thus, AngII levels would control striatal dopaminergic neurotransmission, and would serve to immediately regulate DA function [79]. Moreover, AngII has a modulatory neurochemical effect over DA-mediated behavior, such as the control of movement and reward processing, which might be susceptible to be controlled by pharmacological manipulation of the RAS [70, 72].


Renin–Angiotensin System and Memory


AngII is actively involved in learning and memory processes. There is a broad spectrum of effects described in this peptide over cognitive tasks. This is probably the result of different methodological procedures, with regard to time and route of administration and type of memory evaluated. It has been observed that local administration of AngII on the hippocampus generates disruptive effects on the acquisition and consolidation of memories [82, 83]. Moreover, it has been found that LTP induction is inhibited by the injection of AngII in CA1 region and BLA [84, 85].

On the other hand, in contrast to the previous mentioned results, numerous authors have reported an improvement in the three stages of memory formation after intra-cerebroventricular administration of AngII (Table 14.2) [8689]. AngII facilitates acquisition in active conditioning trials and retention in passive avoidance tests. This facilitator effect of AngII on memory appears to be mediated by central monoamine systems. In addition, it has been found that dopaminergic projection to the nucleus accumbens is involved in the improving effect of angiotensin peptides on recognition memory in rats [96].


Table 14.2
Ang II-induced alteration over memory stages

A419250_1_En_14_Tab2a_HTML.gifA419250_1_En_14_Tab2b_HTML.gif


The different RAS components administration was observed to either improve (+), interfere (−) or have no effect (=) in the performance of rodents in several learning trials: PA passive avoidance, AA active avoidance, CARs conditioned avoidance responses, TL transfer latency


Final Considerations


The functional relation between Amph and AngII activity has been studied over the last decade by this working group. Overall, it has been shown that Amph-induced long-term changes at the reward circuit involve AT1-R activation with regard to responses of the reward circuit. We have observed that AT1-R are involved in the behavioral and neurochemical adaptive responses induced by Amph exposure [5, 97]. Moreover, Amph exposure has been shown to induce long-term changes in AT1-R density and in angiotensinogen mRNA in CPu, a rich DA area strongly related to drugs of abuse responses [98].

Recently, we have shown that AT1-R play a functional role in Amph-induced alterations over neurocognitive processes. In our experimental model, acute Amph impaired memory retention in male rats in the one-trial inhibitory avoidance test when administered immediately post-training. This effect involved central AT1-R activation, since central AT1-R antagonist administration before the psychostimulant partially prevented drug-induced memory impairment [32].

Moreover, a previous experience of repeated Amph followed by 7 days of withdrawal modified the animals’ performance in the inhibitory avoidance test, observed as a resistance to the acute Amph interference effect in the behavioral response. Moreover, these animals showed an altered neuronal activation pattern in BLA after the test session. Long-term Amph-induced alterations were shown in hippocampus synaptic transmission, measured as a lower threshold necessary to generate LTP. It is noteworthy that AT1-R blockade prevented the behavioral, neurochemical, and electrophysiological alterations observed in the repeated Amph group, pointing out a functional role for AT1-R in the psychostimulant-induced neuroadaptations [32].

Brain RAS is a neuromodulatory system of superior brain activities and by its AT1-R can actively modulate Amph-induced alterations. Because AT1-R blockers are currently and safely used in clinics for different pathologies, our results suggest that they would be prominent candidates for pharmacological treatment in pathologies related to altered DA and cognitive alterations.


References



1.

Tulving E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:1–25.CrossRefPubMed


2.

Bruel-Jungerman E, Davis S, Laroche S. Brain plasticity mechanisms and memory: a party of four. Neuroscientiste. 2007;13(5):492–505.CrossRef


3.

Fietta P, Fietta P. The neurobiology of the human memory. Theor Biol Forum. 2011;104(1):69–87.PubMed


4.

Kandel ER, Squire LR. Neuroscience: breaking down scientific barriers to the study of brain and mind. Science. 2000;290(5494):1113–20.CrossRefPubMed


5.

Glannon W. Psychopharmacology and memory. J Med Ethics. 2006;32(2):74–8.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral


6.

Cowan N. What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? Prog Brain Res. 2008;169:323–38.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral


7.

Setlow B. The nucleus accumbens and learning and memory. J Neurosci Res. 1997;49(5):515–21.CrossRefPubMed


8.

Moscovitch M, Rosenbaum RS, Gilboa A, Addis DR, Westmacott R, Grady C, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: a unified account based on multiple trace theory. J Anat. 2005;207(1):35–66.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral


9.

Squire LR. Declarative and nondeclarative memory: multiple brain systems supporting learning and memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 1992;4(3):232–43.CrossRefPubMed


10.

Dudai Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:51–86.CrossRefPubMed


11.

Abel T, Lattal KM. Molecular mechanisms of memory acquisition, consolidation and retrieval. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2001;11(2):180–7.CrossRefPubMed


12.

Dudai Y. Molecular bases of long-term memories: a question of persistence. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12(2):211–6.CrossRefPubMed


13.

McGaugh JL. Memory — a century of consolidation. Science. 2000;287(5451):248–51.CrossRefPubMed


14.

Richter-Levin G, Akirav I. Amygdala–hippocampus dynamic interaction in relation to memory. Mol Neurobiol. 2000;22(1–3):11–20.CrossRefPubMed


15.

Arshavsky YI. “The seven sins” of the Hebbian synapse: can the hypothesis of synaptic plasticity explain long-term memory consolidation? Prog Neurobiol. 2006;80(3):99–113.CrossRefPubMed


16.

Carasatorre M, Ramirez-Amaya V. Network, cellular, and molecular mechanisms underlying long-term memory formation. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2013;15:73–115.CrossRefPubMed


17.

Lisman J, Grace AA, Duzel E. A neoHebbian framework for episodic memory; role of dopamine-dependent late LTP. Trends Neurosci. 2011;34(10):536–47.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral

Oct 20, 2017 | Posted by in PSYCHIATRY | Comments Off on Mechanisms Involved in Memory Processes: Alterations Induced by Psychostimulants—Targeting the Central AT1 Receptors

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access