Chapter 25 Psychogenic movement disorders
Phenomenology, diagnosis, and treatment
Introduction
An entire international symposium on psychogenic movement disorders was held in late 2004, and the subsequent publication of its proceedings is now available (Hallett et al., 2006). That volume covers all aspects of psychogenic movement disorders, beyond the scope of this chapter, and scholars in the field are encouraged to read it. A second symposium was held in April 2009, and its proceedings will be published soon.
Psychogenic movement disorders are caused by psychological factors rather than by an organic etiology. Other terms such as functional, nonorganic, and medically unexplained symptoms have been used. Although the term functional might be more convenient to convey to patients and their families – because of old stigmas regarding having a psychological disorder – the term psychogenic describes the condition best because it refers to the etiology. This is much the same way neurologists label many disorders, such as postencephalitic parkinsonism, vascular parkinsonism, post-traumatic parkinsonism or drug-induced parkinsonism. Why not label parkinsonism due to psychogenic etiology as psychogenic parkinsonism? It places the emphasis on etiology and thereby guides the physician towards appropriate treatment. The term functional has been used in the past to denote organic diseases in which a specific cause was not determined, and has been applied to organic illnesses such as chorea, epilepsy, and neuralgias because these were considered physiologic, rather than anatomic, disorders (see historical reviews by Fahn, 2006a; Munts and Koehler, 2010). Today, for most clinicians, the meaning of functional is synonymous with psychogenic, but this might not be uniformly defined this way, so it would be less ambiguous to use psychogenic. The term organic is used to mean “not due to a psychogenic etiology,” and, thus, the term nonorganic refers to psychogenic. More and more the literature in neurology has adopted the term psychogenic, as can be seen by the epilepsy specialty. The terminology debate in epilepsy appears to be not about psychogenic terminology, but about whether the episodes should be called psychogenic nonepileptiform seizures or attacks (Benbadis, 2010; LaFrance, 2010).
Frequency
Psychogenic disorders in neurology have been estimated to occur in 1–9% of all neurologic diagnoses (Marsden, 1986; Lempert et al., 1990). Neurologists usually and appropriately recognize patients with psychogenic movement disorders, but the patients often do not accept this diagnosis and seek other opinions, going from physician to physician, seeking a diagnosis that is more to their liking. Therefore, a strategy is necessary for the best way to inform the patients of the diagnosis. This issue is discussed in this chapter. Another common situation is that many physicians do not offer the time-consuming care that is necessary to restore such patients to normality, preferring instead merely to diagnose the condition and have the referring physician deal with the healing.
Like other subspecialties in neurology, psychogenic movement disorders are not uncommon. In one large movement disorder clinic, such patients account for 10% of all non-parkinsonian new patient visits (Portera-Cailliau et al., 2006). Typically, patients are diagnosed by the predominant movement feature, e.g., psychogenic tremor, psychogenic dystonia, psychogenic myoclonus, etc. When evaluated this way, tremor is the most common psychogenic phenomenology, followed by dystonia.
Importance of an accurate diagnosis
The diagnosis of a psychogenic movement disorder is a two-stage process (Lang, 2006). First is to make a positive diagnosis that the movements are psychogenic and not due to an organic illness. Second is to identify a psychiatric disorder or the psychodynamics that could explain the etiology of the abnormal movements and prepare the way to deciding the best course for therapy of the individual patient. Deciding between abnormal movements due to a psychogenic cause and an organic one can be extremely difficult. Never having seen strange movements before and pronouncing them to be psychogenic therefore is not a satisfactory method because not even senior movement disorder specialists have seen the whole gamut of organic abnormal movements. An organic cause of the movements needs to be excluded (Fahn, 1994; Williams et al., 2005). But this alone is insufficient and neurologists in times past and present advise that making a diagnosis of a psychogenic disorder depends on finding positive criteria and not simply failing to finding an organic cause (for historical review, see Fahn 2006a).
If the obverse occurs – that is, if a patient is given a diagnosis of a psychogenic movement disorder when, in fact, he or she suffers from an organic one – again the wrong treatment is given. In this situation, time-consuming and expensive psychotherapy, psychiatric medications, and possibly electroconvulsive therapy might be initiated instead of more appropriate pharmacotherapeutic agents that could provide relief. Moreover, a diagnosis of a psychogenic disorder can create emotional trauma for the patient and his or her family (Cooper, 1976). It is important to point out that no matter how much experience a clinician has had, encountering a new type of movement disorder for the first time does not automatically make this a psychogenic movement disorder. For example, task-specific jaw tremor (Miles et al., 1997) is rare, and even when it is encountered for the first time, a wise clinician should consider it to be organic (Video 25.1) and not psychogenic (Video 25.2).
Neurologic symptoms and signs are a common result of hysteria, and neurologists have long been fascinated by the brain’s ability to be able to produce such clinical expressions on the basis of psychological disturbances. Many great neurologists, such as Charcot and Freud, intensively studied hysterical conversion reactions, using hypnosis as a tool in their investigations and treatment (Goetz, 1987). In their training, neurologists-to-be are taught to differentiate the clinical findings of psychogenic etiology from those of organic disorders (Gowers, 1893; Oppenheim, 1911a; DeJong, 1958a). However, textbooks in the past often considered some dyskinesias, recognized today as organic, such as tics, writer’s cramp and other occupational cramps, and some other forms of dystonia, to be examples of hysteria (DeJong, 1958b).
Although there is a modest neurologic literature on psychogenic phenomenology, the literature dealing specifically with psychogenic movement disorders is rather sparse. For example, tremor as the result of a conversion reaction has been long recognized, at least since the days of Gowers (1893), but scientific reports on psychogenic tremor or other movement disorders are rarely described in the literature. Campbell (1979) pointed out that the amplitude of psychogenic tremor is more pronounced when attention is paid to it, and that it lessens and may even disappear when the patient’s attention is diverted to another subject or other part of the body. Distraction is a useful part of the examination, and lessening of severity by distraction can be very helpful diagnostically in trying to establish a diagnosis of psychogenic tremor. But in our experience, distraction does not always succeed in making the tremor disappear, so this maneuver is often not successful. Therefore, additional findings on examination are often necessary and can be just as helpful in considering the diagnosis of a psychogenic movement disorder; these are discussed in this chapter.
Although in the great majority of patients with a psychogenic movement disorder, all their clinical features result only from a psychogenic problem, some may have the psychogenic movement disorder on top of an organic movement disorder, as seen in Patient 5 in the series of psychogenic dystonias reported by Fahn and Williams (1988) and the cases of Ranawaya and colleagues (1990). Perhaps 10–15% of patients with a psychogenic movement disorder have an underlying organic movement disorder as well. This overlap is seen in patients with psychogenic seizures (“pseudoseizures”); 10–37% of patients have organic seizures as well (Krumholz and Niedermeyer, 1983; Lesser et al., 1983). Nevertheless, a useful rule of thumb is that if one part of the examination reveals nonorganicity, it is likely that other “abnormalities” on the examination might also be nonorganic.
Perhaps the movement disorders with the highest prevalence rate of a psychogenic origin are the nonfamilial, “idiopathic”, paroxysmal nonkinesigenic dyskinesias, as surveyed by Bressman and colleagues (1988). They found that of 18 patients with paroxysmal nonkinesigenic dystonias and with no known symptomatic etiology or positive family history for a paroxysmal dyskinesia, the dystonias were due to psychogenic causes in 11 patients. This represents 61% of such cases. The age at onset in these patients ranged from 11 to 49 years; 8 of the 11 patients were female. Thus, unless accompanied by a clear-cut family history, these paroxysmal dystonias are commonly psychogenic, and their diagnosis is extremely difficult to make for reasons that are explained later in the chapter (Fahn and Williams, 1988).
Kotagal and colleagues (2002) conducted a study of paroxysmal events in children. Over a 6-year period, 883 patients were monitored in their pediatric epilepsy monitoring unit; 134 patients (15.2%) were documented to have paroxysmal nonepileptic events. Children in the preschool group (age: 2 months to 5 years) (n = 26) were eventually diagnosed with stereotypies, hypnic jerks, parasomnias, and Sandifer syndrome. The school-age group (age: 5–12 years) (n = 61) had diagnoses of psychogenic seizures, inattention or daydreaming, stereotypies, hypnic jerks, and paroxysmal dyskinesias. The adolescent group (age: 12–18 years) (n = 48) had a diagnosis of psychogenic seizures in 40 patients (83%). The authors concluded that in patients with paroxysmal nonepileptic events, conversion disorder was seen in children older than 5 years of age, and its frequency increased with age, becoming the most common type of paroxysmal nonepileptic events among adolescents. In adolescents, conversion disorder was more common in females, whereas males predominated in the school-age group. Concomitant epilepsy with nonepileptic events occurred in all three age groups to a varying extent.
Mass hysteria
Today, neurologists encounter individual patients with psychogenic movement disorders. But historically, mass hysteria was common, probably more so than it is today. Mass hysteria still occurs, such as “shell shock” in wartime, as well as during current environmental events such as mass inoculations (Kharabsheh et al., 2001; Khiem et al., 2003). Symptoms can also be generated from mass concerns about medications and breast implants, in part owing to widespread publicity, although legal liability issues may also drive the development of symptoms. Mass hysteria resembling seizures occurred recently in 10 high school girls following the development of organic absence seizures in another student (Roach and Langley, 2004).
It is interesting to note on a historical level that the term chorea, meaning dancing in Latin, comes from the dancing mania (a mass hysteria) that was seen in the Middle Ages, and from which the term St Vitus’ dance was coined; this term subsequently was applied by Sydenham to describe the condition now referred to as Sydenham chorea (Hayden, 1981).
For further information, see the 2001 monograph by Halligan and colleagues, which is devoted to the topic of hysteria.
Physiologic basis for psychogenic neurologic dysfunctions – neuroimaging
It is intellectually intriguing that the brain can create neurologic deficits – such as paralysis, sensory loss, blindness, seizures, and movement disorders – from psychological factors. This mysterious ability fascinated pioneers working on hysteria, such as Charcot and Freud. We have not been much enlightened over the ensuing 100-plus years until recently, when newer imaging technologies have shed some light on the mechanism. As stated by Hallett (2010), physiologically, we cannot tell the difference between voluntary and involuntary. The exceptions are electrophysiologic measurements of myoclonus, startle, tremor and blepharospasm. The fast 50–100 ms contractions of organic myoclonus cannot be duplicated by psychogenic myoclonus. Psychogenic startle responses are too delayed and variable to be a physiologic reflex (Thompson et al., 1992). Psychogenic tremor can often be detected by variable width on spiral analysis (Hsu et al., 2009) and by other tests like coactivation sign and tapping (Deuschl et al., 1998, 2006; Zeuner et al., 2003; Criswell et al., 2010). The R2 recovery in the blink reflex is disinhibited in organic blepharospasm and is normal in psychogenic blepharospasm (Schwingenschuh et al., 2011). The Bereitschaftspotential (BP) (readiness potential) is seen with ordinarily voluntary movements, but by itself does not indicate that a movement is voluntary (Hallett, 2010). The electrophysiology of psychogenic dystonia resembles that of organic dystonia (Espay et al., 2006; Quartarone et al., 2009), except for findings of increased plasticity in the latter.
Vuilleumier and colleagues (2001) utilized single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) to measure regional cerebral blood flow with and without bilateral vibration in seven patients with unilateral psychogenic sensory loss and then again in four subjects after full recovery. These studies revealed a consistent decrease of regional cerebral blood flow in the thalamus and basal ganglia contralateral to the deficit, which reverted to normal when the patients recovered. These results suggest that hysterical conversion deficits might entail a functional disorder in striatothalamocortical circuits controlling sensorimotor function and voluntary motor behavior. The same subcortical premotor circuits are involved in unilateral motor neglect after organic neurologic damage.
In an analogous situation, a patient who was diagnosed with psychogenic reduced visual acuity underwent a SPECT scan that revealed reduced regional cerebral blood flow in the bilateral visual association areas but not in the primary visual areas (Okuyama et al., 2002). The authors concluded that psychogenic visual disturbance is associated with functional suppression of the visual association area. A listing of functional imaging studies carried out in hysteria has been collated by Fink and colleagues (2006). These studies indicate that alterations in regional brain activity may accompany the expression of conversion symptoms. Since then, another study utilizing positron emission tomography (PET) in patients with hysterical anesthesia showed lack of response in the contralateral cortex to sensory stimulation of the affected limb, again indicating the brain physiologically shuts down as a result of the hysterical anesthesia (Ghaffar et al., 2006). Yet when bilateral stimulation is applied, there is bilateral activation of the cortex, indicating that the cortex is indeed intact.
The results in functional imaging studies are complemented by motor physiology studies. When cortical and spinal inhibition was evaluated in psychogenic and organic dystonia, both groups had similar results of reduced inhibition (Espay et al., 2006). Again, this suggests that the central nervous system accommodates its physiology to follow the motor pattern, or the reverse, i.e., that the abnormal psychological problem alters brain physiology, which then produces the abnormal motoric effect.
Degree of certainty of the diagnosis of a psychogenic movement disorder
Fahn and Williams (1988) categorized patients into four levels of certainty as to the likelihood of their having psychogenic dystonia; these categories can be applied to all psychogenic movement disorders. These four degrees of certainty are: (1) documented psychogenic disorder, (2) clinically established psychogenic disorder, (3) probable psychogenic disorder, and (4) possible psychogenic movement disorder. Subsequent authors have used this classification (Koller et al., 1989; Ranawaya et al., 1990; Lang, 1995; Shill and Gerber, 2006). We have incorporated the classification of Fahn and Williams but have expanded the criteria somewhat, taking into account additional observations since the original publication.
Documented psychogenic movement disorder
Just being suspicious that the signs and symptoms are psychogenic is insufficient for the diagnosis of documented psychogenic disorder. For the disorder to be documented as being psychogenic, the symptoms must be completely relieved by psychotherapy, by the clinician utilizing psychological suggestion including physiotherapy, or by administration of placebos (again with suggestion being a part of this approach), or the patient must be witnessed as being free of symptoms when left alone, supposedly unobserved. This last feature would be a major factor in proving psychogenicity in those who are malingering or have a factitious disorder, since such patients would not likely obtain relief of symptoms by manipulation of the examiner. Insurance companies are increasingly using videotaped surveillance to protect themselves against fraud. There is now a published report of a patient with a clinical diagnosis of reflex sympathetic dystrophy who sued for financial compensation despite features of psychogenicity and was videotaped while under surveillance by a private investigator to be completely free of the abnormal movements, allowing the case to be settled for a reduced amount (Kurlan et al., 1997). If the signs and symptoms disappear and do not return, that is fairly good evidence that the underlying psychiatric problem has been relieved. But it is not uncommon for the psychogenic movement disorder to return if the patient does not obtain complete relief of the psychiatric factors that led to the neurologic dysfunction.
A critical issue for using the relief of signs as a criterion for the definition is that most organic movement disorders rarely remit spontaneously and completely except for tics, tardive dyskinesia, infectious (e.g., Sydenham chorea), and drug-induced reactions, and rarely, essential myoclonus (Fahn and Sjaastad, 1991). Other organic disorders, such as Parkinson disease, Huntington disease, and essential tremor, are persistent and even progressive. Idiopathic torsion dystonia, except for torticollis (Jayne et al., 1984; Friedman and Fahn, 1986; Jahanshahi et al., 1990), rarely totally remits. On occasion, patients with other types of dystonia will show improvement, but this improvement is typically incomplete and temporary (Marsden and Harrison, 1974), although gradual, prolonged, incomplete improvement has been encountered in at least one patient (Eldridge et al., 1984).
Clinically established psychogenic movement disorder
When the movement disorder is inconsistent over time (the features are different when the patient is observed at subsequent examinations) or is incongruent with a classic movement disorder, one becomes suspicious that the movements are psychogenic. If either inconsistency or incongruity is present and, in addition the patient has any of the manifestations listed in Table 25.1, one can feel comfortable in believing that the disorder is psychogenic, and this has been referred to as clinically established psychogenic movement disorder.
1 Other neurologic signs are present that are definitely psychogenic (e.g., false weakness, false sensory findings, and self-inflicted injuries) |
It should be noted that item 4 in Table 25.1 by itself is insufficient for a diagnosis of a documented or clinically established psychogenic disorder. This is because some organic movement disorders can be temporarily and voluntarily suppressed (Koller and Biary, 1989). Similarly, akathitic movements and paradoxical dystonia (Fahn, 1989) also tend to disappear with active voluntary movement. The difficulty establishing psychogenic paroxysmal dyskinesias which have a normal interictal pattern is covered below. Fortunately, myoclonus has such a short-duration contraction that it cannot be purposely produced, so electrophysiologic evidence against organic myoclonus could be one of the factors along with inconsistency and incongruity that can lead to a clinically established diagnosis of a psychogenic myoclonus. Psychogenic tremors have features such as a coactivation sign (Deuschl et al., 1998, 2006) or a change in tremor frequency due to entrainment by tapping the contralateral limb (Zeuner et al., 2003). These features would establish the tremor as psychogenic.
Probable psychogenic movement disorder
This definition contains four categories of patients:
Definitions of psychiatric terminology
Somatoform disorder
The abnormal movements of somatoform disorders are involuntary. The knowledge that one is the cause of one’s own actions is known as self-agency. Thus, people with abnormal movements due to a somatoform disorder lack self-agency. The right inferior parietal cortex is the brain area believed to play a role in self-agency. Voon and her colleagues (2010) utilized functional magnetic resonance imaging in eight subjects with a conversion disorder (tremor). The right temporoparietal junction was found to be hypoactive compared to those purposefully mimicking the same type of tremor (P < 0.05). There was an associated reduced connectivity between this cortical area and the sensorimotor areas and the limbic system.
Clues suggesting the presence of a psychogenic movement disorder
Often, there are clues from the history and neurologic examination that lead the clinician to suspect a diagnosis of psychogenic dystonia. Fahn and Williams (1988), Koller and colleagues (1989), and Factor and colleagues (1995) have enunciated many of these clues. Tables 25.2 and 25.3 list these clues, as well as some additional ones.
Abrupt onset of the movement disorder is quite common. The presence of more than one type of dyskinesia (item 5 in Table 25.2) is another important clue, often confounding clinicians (Video 25.3). Most of the time, patients with an organic movement disorder present with only a single type. A note of caution is warranted, however. Certain disorders progress to involve more than one type of abnormal movement. For example, Huntington disease can have chorea, bradykinesia, dystonia, and myoclonus (Penney et al., 1990). Neuroacanthocytosis often is manifested by both chorea and tics (Hardie et al., 1991). Some patients with childhood-onset tics may later develop torsion dystonia (Shale et al., 1986; Stone and Jankovic, 1991). Often, patients with idiopathic torsion dystonia will have dystonic tremor or rapid movements that resemble myoclonus or chorea (Fahn et al., 1987). Patients with tardive dyskinesia may have a combination of rhythmical oral-buccal-lingual movements plus dystonia and akathitic movements (Fahn, 1984). Table 1.17 in Chapter 1 lists the diseases that are composed of multiple types of movement disorders.
The most common types of movements, whether isolated or in the presence of other types of movements, in patients with psychogenic movement disorders are shaking movements (item 4 in Table 25.2) that can resemble organic tremors or peculiar, atypical tremors (Video 25.4). This was the most common type of abnormal movement in the spectrum of psychogenic movement disorders reported by Lang (2006) and by Jankovic et al. (2006b). Another note of caution about unusual tremors is that Wilson disease can also present with unusual tremors (Shale et al., 1987, 1988). Utterances of gibberish can be another useful sign (Videos 25.5 and 25.6).
Item 4 in Table 25.2 also lists bizarre gaits as a feature of psychogenicity. After tremors and dystonia, bizarre gait disorders are the next most common type of unusual movement disorder encountered in patients with mixed features of movements. The psychogenic gait can show posturing, excessive slowness, and hesitation. There could be pseudo-ataxia or careful walking (like walking on ice). The latter resembles the fear-of-falling syndrome, which is discussed in more detail later. There can be sudden buckling of a leg, as if there is weakness (Vecht et al., 1991), but the failure to produce this dipping movement each time the patient steps on the leg would be evidence of inconsistency. Accompanying this bizarre gait would be a variability of impairment and excessive swaying when tested for the Romberg sign, without actually falling.
Another aspect of item 4 is excessive startle that could mimic hyperekplexia, excessive startle syndrome, the jumping Frenchmen syndrome (Video 25.7), or even reflex myoclonus (Andermann and Andermann, 1986). Thompson and colleagues (1992) determined the physiologic parameters that are typically seen in patients with psychogenic startles. There is a variable latency to the onset of the jerk; the latencies are greater than those seen in reflex myoclonus of cortical or brainstem origin; the latencies are longer than the fastest voluntary reaction time; there are variable patterns of muscle recruitment within each jerk; and there is significant habituation with repeated stimulation. This last point is probably not specific to psychogenic startle, since organic startles may also show habituation. Noise stimuli can induce other abnormal movements besides startle. Walters and Hening (1992) reported a case with psychogenic tremor following sudden, loud noise; the patient had a post-traumatic stress disorder.
Item 11 in Table 25.2 points out that in a patient with psychogenic tremor, when asked to carry out rapid successive movements, such as tapping the index finger on the thumb, the rate of the tremor becomes the same as the rapid successive movements (i.e., the tremor has become entrained). In contrast, an organic tremor is the dominant rate; it will gradually force the voluntary movements to be the same rate as the tremor.
Idiopathic torsion dystonia usually begins with action dystonia (Fahn et al., 1987), but psychogenic dystonia often begins with a fixed posture. Fixed postures are sustained postures that resist passive movement, and the presence of such fixed postures is highly likely to be due to a psychogenic dystonia (Fahn and Williams, 1988; Lang and Fahn, 1990; Schrag et al., 2004; Schrag, 2006). The posture can manifest so much rigidity that it is extremely difficult to move the limb about a joint (Video 25.8). Often, the psychogenic dystonia resembles reflex sympathetic dystrophy (complex regional pain syndrome, CRPS) because there is accompanying pain, tenderness (allodynia), and skin changes (Lang and Fahn, 1990; Schwartzman and Kerrigan, 1990; Bhatia et al., 1993; Schrag et al., 2004; Schrag, 2006). Nerve injury leading to pain, shiny red skin, and fixed postures was called causalgia by Mitchell et al. (1864). Charcot (1892) considered the disorder hysterical (see Munts and Koehler, 2010). The term “reflex sympathetic dystrophy” was coined by Evans (1946) because the phenotype could occur in the absence of trauma to a major nerve and might be due to sympathetic nerves. “Complex regional pain syndrome” was the recommended term in 1994, with type 1 being reflex sympathetic dystrophy and type 2 being causalgia (Merskey and Bogduk, 1994).
To make matters confusing, in many cases, psychogenic dystonia of a limb follows a minor trauma to that limb, similar to the pattern of reflex sympathetic dystrophy. On the other hand, organic dystonia of a body part can be preceded by an injury to that body part (Schott, 1985, 1986; Scherokman et al., 1986; Gordon et al., 1990; Goldman and Ahlskog, 1993), so it can be difficult to distinguish between organic and psychogenic dystonia. Fixed painful postural torticollis following trauma is not uncommon, and determining whether it is organic or psychogenic is difficult, but analysis indicates that many of these cases appear to be psychogenic in etiology (Sa et al., 2003).
Although a fixed dystonic posture, in which the joint cannot be passively extended or flexed, is often psychogenic, it could also be due to a contracture (which, of course could also be the result of a psychogenic dystonia as well as an organic dystonia). The evaluation of such fixed postures in which the affected joint cannot be passively altered requires the aid of anesthesia to see if contractures are present (Fahn, 2006b). This technique not only aids the diagnosis but also guides the clinician and patient in what to expect during therapy (Video 25.9). The prognosis of fixed dystonia is often poor (Ibrahim et al., 2009), but it depends on recognizing this disorder as likely to be a psychogenic one and then applying treatment according the principles described below.
Debates continue as to whether CRPS is organic or psychogenic. This topic was discussed at the Second Psychogenic Movement Disorder Symposium in 2009. A recent proposal that small fiber neuropathy might be responsible (Oaklander and Fields, 2009) has been countered by the observation that the patient had the phenomenology of a psychogenic dystonia (Lang and Chen, 2010).
In addition to the clues in the history and examination related to the movements themselves presented in Table 25.2, there are often clues in the non-movement history and examination that help the clinician consider a psychogenic disorder. These are presented in Table 25.3.
We have been impressed by the frequency with which patients with psychogenic movement disorders are employed in some capacity working in the health profession (item 6 in Table 25.3). Many are nurses; another large group are employed in the health insurance industry, processing medical claims.
Another point that is worth commenting on is that many of the affected individuals have a devoted spouse who responds readily to their pressing needs (item 7 in Table 25.3). Some spouses carry a pager so that the patient can easily call them while they are at work. Others pamper the patient, who might even be wheelchair bound, especially if the patient has psychogenic dystonia. Such devotion could be a secondary gain from having the movement disorder.