Stress

and Penney Upton2



(1)
Faculty of Health, University of Canberra, Canberra, Aust Capital Terr, Australia

(2)
Institute of Health and Society, University of Worcester, Worcester, UK

 




Box 3.1: Key Points





  • Stress can have a significant impact on wound healing;


  • Stress can be a consequence of the wound, social isolation, psychological issues, pain from the wound and pain from the treatment regimen;


  • The interactional model of stress suggests stress is a transaction between an individual and their environment – an event only elicits a stress response if the individual perceives the event to be stressful;


  • The General Adaptation Syndrome (GAS) describes three stages associated with stress: alarm, resistance, and exhaustion stage;


  • The importance of perception in the understanding of stress and how this can be applied to wound treatment is emphasised.


Summary


The relationship between stress and delayed healing is now firmly established. This evidenced relationship may be associated with a range of factors linked to wound care. For example, the novelty of certain treatments- negative pressure for example- or the pain associated with wound management. Given the range of factors that are associated with wound related stress it is important that the health care professional understands the nature and model of stress, how best to assess it in practice and how any stress can be managed. These will be the foci of this particular chapter. Stress may be a consequence of several wound related factors: most particularly pain and wound management pain in particular. Preventing stress, and stress related to pain, is important not just for the relationship with delayed wound healing, but importantly, to understand that this may be an ongoing deleterious cyclical relationship. Increased wound related pain leads to increased stress which leads to delayed healing, which can lead to more pain. It is this cycle that this chapter addresses and suggests methods to break this damaging predicament.


Introduction


Many clinicians, when asked, believe that there is a relationship between stress and delayed healing (Upton and Solowiej 2011). It is evident from the research literature that there is strong empirical support for this belief (Broadbent et al. 2003; Ebrecht et al. 2004; Francis and Pennebaker 1992; Gouin et al. 2008; Jones et al. 2006; Marucha et al. 1998; Weinman et al. 2008; Kiecolt-Glaser et al. 1995; Glaser et al. 1999; Cole-King and Harding 2001) with a major theme suggesting that interventions need to be implemented as part of the wound care process in order to minimise patients’ psychological stress/anxiety. This delayed healing may be evident in both acute and chronic wounds. For example, wound healing is a critical outcome in acute surgical wounds (Broadbent et al. 2003). Poor healing can result in wound infections or complications, as well as prolonged hospital stays, increased patient discomfort, and delayed return to activity (Broadbent et al. 2003). It is therefore important to maximise the healing rate by minimising any stress (see Box 3.1).

In the Broadbent et al. study (2003), a sample of 47 patients with an inguinal hernia were given a standardised questionnaire assessing psychological stress and worry about the operation before undergoing open incision repair. Greater worry about the operation predicted more painful, poorer and slower recovery. These results also suggest that in clinical practice, interventions to reduce the patient’s psychological stress may improve wound repair and healing, and facilitate recovery following surgery.

A similar pattern, albeit for different reasons, exists with chronic wounds. Psychological stress can be experienced in the presence of pain in chronic wounds so that the pain itself is seen as a stressor. Stress can also be induced by the anticipation of pain, for example as a result of waiting to have a dressing changed (Soon and Acton 2006). Thus, pain, or anticipation of pain associated with treatment itself, may have detrimental effects on the process of chronic wound healing (see Fig. 3.1). In a series of studies Upton and colleagues suggested a link between pain, stress and healing in those with a chronic wound (e.g. Upton 2011a, b; Solowiej et al. 2009, 2010a, b). In particular, the evidence suggested that the stress of dressing change was a significant factor in those with a chronic wound.

A320414_1_En_3_Fig1_HTML.jpg


Figure 3.1
A model illustrating link between psychological stress, pain and chronic wound healing

This overview has indicated that stress may have an important part to play in both acute and chronic wound healing and hence it is important for the practitioner to be aware of stress, its definition and measurement and the potential impact it has on healing along with potential remediation strategies. Although there are many complex models of stress, there are two fundamental theories: The General Adaptation Syndrome (GAS) and the Interactional Model. These will be explored next since they underpin some of the subsequent observations and interventions in wound care.


General Adaption Syndrome (GAS) Model of Stress


The physiological model of stress- the General Adaption Syndrome (GAS) was proposed by Selye (1956). When a patient is experiencing stress there are usually a number of common bodily responses that can be identified, including; increased respiration, blood pressure or heart rate. These responses are all part of the ‘fight or flight’ syndrome that prepares the body and which are mediated through the nervous and endocrine system. The GAS describes three stages that are evident within the stress process, consisting of: (1) Alarm stage; (2) Resistance stage; (3) Exhaustion stage:



  • Alarm stage: whereby the body prepares for the stressor by mobilising its available resources. This stage echo’s that of the ‘fight or flight’ response in that the body’s resources are mobilised for action. Nonetheless, if the stressor is persistent, resources become depleted leading to fatigue and subsequently triggers the resistance stage.


  • Resistance stage: here the body adapts to the stressor, allowing physiological arousal to decline but still remaining higher than normal. It is here that the individual becomes at risk of health implications due to impaired immune functioning. Consequently, the exhaustion stage is triggered.


  • Exhaustion stage: When an individual’s body is no longer able to respond and recover and resistance are no longer possible, a state of exhaustion will occur. This stage is highly detrimental and can lead to death.

The GAS was one of the first models of stress, being underpinned by the classic work of Cannon (1932): revealing that when an individual is stressed, physiological systems are activated in order to prepare for ‘fight of flight’ (Kemeny 2003). Although this process is beneficial to the individual in the short term, it can become highly maladaptive if the stressor becomes long term. Selye (1956), on the basis of the ‘fight or flight’ response, observed a number of animals and their reaction to stressful situations, discovering that this reaction was the beginning of a series of responses made by the body and ultimately physiological collapse, or exhaustion (see Fig. 3.2).

A320414_1_En_3_Fig2_HTML.jpg


Figure 3.2
Model of the general adaption syndrome

This prolonged experience of stress can significantly lower the immune functioning and the lower levels of various inflammatory cytokines and enzymes that are influential in tissue repair (Upton 2011a, b), and in the case of wound care, delayed wound healing. Such changes in cytokine and enzyme levels could explain the relationship between reduced wound healing and stress (Marucha et al. 1998). Physiologically, prolonged stress can lead to raised levels of the hormone cortisol. Although stress isn’t the only reason that cortisol is secreted into the bloodstream, it has been termed “the stress hormone” because it is secreted in higher levels during the body’s ‘fight or flight’ response to stress (Ebrecht et al. 2004). Higher and more prolonged levels of cortisol in the bloodstream have been shown to have negative effects on the body including, increased heart rate, higher blood pressure and lowered immunity and inflammatory responses in the body.

Psychologically, stress can increase the likelihood of patients making cognitive errors or negative appraisals, for example perceiving a dressing removal as an unpleasant experience, which can result in detrimental effects to the wound healing process and/or avoidance of treatment. Thus negative emotional responses affect biological and behavioural responses which feed back to further negatively affect the emotional response to pain, producing a continuous cycle (Adams et al. 2006) with delayed wound healing as a consequence. It is important for clinicians to recognise that pain and stress are both comprised of complex interactions between physiological, psychological and social factors (see Table 3.1).


Table 3.1
Examples of psychological, biological and social factors contributing to both stress and pain




























Psychological factors

Biological factors

Social factors

Negative emotional associations (e.g. anticipation of pain at dressing change)

Fight or flight response – sympathetic arousal

Availability and quality of social support

Previous experience of pain and stress

Cortisol released

Quality of personal relationships

Individual differences in perception/appraisal of a stressful event

Increase in heart rate, breathing rate, blood pressure

Social comparisons with other patients at different stages of recovery

Coping strategies

Lowered immune system functioning

Environmental factors (e.g. hospital/clinic attendance vs. home visits)

Although the GAS model provided an insight into the physiological stress process, it regards the individual as responding to a stressor, with stress being a linear stimulus-response framework. In doing so it ignores the role of psychological factors and individual differences. Hence, Lazarus and Folkman (1984, 1987) developed an alternative model that integrated the potential for psychological variables: the interactional model of stress.


Interactional Model of Stress


Within the interactional model of stress, Lazarus and Folkman (1984, 1987) posited that stress and stress perception was based upon a transaction between an individual and their external world/environment. Hence, a stressful event would only elicit a stress response if the individual actually perceived the event to be stressful. In doing so, this model accounts for the individual differences that may be evident in perceiving an event stressful (E.g. while one patient may perceive a dressing change to be highly stressful, another may perceive it as a usual occurrence; see Fig. 3.3).

A320414_1_En_3_Fig3_HTML.jpg


Figure 3.3
The interactional model of stress (Based on Lazarus and Folkman (1984, 1987))

As can be seen in Fig. 3.3, this model proposes two types of appraisal; primary and secondary. The primary appraisal, whereby the event is appraised as to whether it is a threat or not (see Rovira et al. 2010a, b; Schlotz et al. 2011), is the initial response to a particular demand. From this, then two possible outcomes are available: either a threat or not (and the latter may either be positive, or neutral). If an individual perceives the situation to be threatening, secondary appraisal occurs; where the individual’s ability to cope and the resources available are assessed (see Prati et al. 2010) and put into action. The available choices from this assessment reflects the control that the patient may have; taking control of the situation themself, gaining help from another, or avoiding the situation (this can be particularly influential when considering patient’s treatment concordance, discussed later in this book). When an individual believes that, given their secondary appraisal, they have limited resources to cope with the situation, they will experience some form of stress and ultimately negative physiological consequences.

From this description of the model, a number of considerations for the wound care clinician can be noted. Firstly, clinicians cannot simply characterise a situation as stressful or not. Due to the differences in stress perception it may be that while one patient perceives a routine visit from the tissue viability nurse as stressful, another may perceive it as benign (or indeed, positive). Similarly, although the nurse may consider the dressing change as routine, normal and hence benign, the patient may not share this view. Furthermore, a patient whom may have experienced a previous painful experience during dressing removal may become highly stressed and anxious: their appraisal is based on learnt behaviour. Therefore, it is necessary for clinicians to be aware of such situations and consider patients on an individual basis.

Something which the clinician may view as routine clinical practice may be considered highly stressful by a patient (Upton 2011a, b). Secondly, the interactional model is based upon cognitive appraisal and is influenced by other psychological factors. Hence, it is possible for a patient to interpret the same situation in a number of differing ways depending on their mood- their cognitive appraisal is modified by their emotional state. Finally, it is important to remember that a patient can experience a stressful response irrespective of whether the event was experienced, anticipated or imagined. Within this model, the mere thought of having a dressing change itself can be stressful for the patient (Upton 2011a, b).

Lazarus and Folkman (1984) suggested two predominant functions of coping; the alteration of the situation (problem-focused coping), or regulation of emotional responses to the situation (emotional-focused coping). Briefly, problem-focused coping aims to reduce the stressful demands of expanding resources associated with dealing with that demand (e.g. this could include avoidance of dressing changes due to the stress perceived beforehand and during the procedure, or adopting a distraction strategy in order to help deal with the perceived stress). Alternatively, emotional-focused coping aims to regulate the emotional response through behavioural coping or cognitive coping strategies. For example, trying to stay calm throughout the treatment and reducing their own stress or worry.


Assessing Stress


It is well documented that psychological stress can result in delayed wound healing (Broadbent et al. 2003; Ebrecht et al. 2004; Francis and Pennebaker 1992; Gouin et al. 2008; Jones et al. 2006; Marucha et al. 1998; Weinman et al. 2008). Also, as highlighted previously in this chapter, stress can result in distorted cognition and subsequent increased pain. Hence, it is essential for wound care professionals to assess patient’s psychological stress throughout the treatment regime. Self-report or physiological measures should be used to assess patient’s stress in addition to an awareness of patient’s behavioural (see Table 3.2) indicators of stress.


Table 3.2
Behavioural signs of stress





















Rapid breathing rate.

Faster eye-blink rate.

Increased heart rate.

Muscle tension.

Squirming, sweating palms.

Dry mouth, tense voice.

Pale skin, cold sweat.

Avoidance behaviour.

In order for clinicians to assess levels of stress and pain in patients with chronic wounds, it is important to recognise that both stress and pain are biopsychosocial concepts (i.e. both stress and pain are comprised of complex interactions between biological, psychological and social components) and hence both physiological and psychological measures are required.

Effective psychological measures of stress focus upon the emotional responses obtained through self-report methods (see Table 3.3 for brief overview). Self-report methods of measuring stress are common in investigating patients’ response to illness/injury (e.g. chronic wounds) and can range from a simple “stress-thermometer” (for example, see Fig. 3.4) to more formal psychometric measures. For example, the PSS allows clinicians to assess a patient’s psychological appraisal of stress, whereas a measurement of increased heart rate or blood pressure would show a physiological outcome of stress. Accurate assessments of this nature allow us to determine the impact of interventions on psychological stress and anxiety in such situations. An overview of some of the more popular psychological measures of stress is presented here.


Table 3.3
An evaluation of a selection of psychological measures of stress



























Measure

Purpose

Strengths

Weaknesses

The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), (Zigmond and Snaith 1983)

Designed for use in medical out-patient clinics to detect clinical cases of anxiety and depression and to assess the severity of anxiety and depression.

Measures state anxiety and depression in a specific clinical setting. Short scale (14-items), that takes approx 10 min for the patient to complete.

Items are designed to measure state anxiety rather than stress. However, anxiety is an outcome of stress.

The State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), (Spielberger 1977)

Designed to measure state and trait anxiety in adults. Clearly differentiates the temporary condition of “state anxiety” and the more general long-term “trait anxiety”.

The state aspect of the questionnaire can determine anxiety in a specific situation (i.e. at dressing change) rather than in a more general sense. It can be used with clinical patients. Includes a 40-item scale which takes approx 10 min to complete.
 

The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), (Cohen and Hoberman 1983)

Designed to measure the degree to which situations in a person’s life are perceived as stressful. Higher scores indicate more perceived stress.

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

May 28, 2017 | Posted by in PSYCHOLOGY | Comments Off on Stress

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access