The Effect of 17β-Estradiol and Its Analogues on Cognition in Preclinical and Clinical Research: Relevance to Schizophrenia

, Maarten van den Buuse2 and Andrea Gogos 



(1)
Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, University of Melbourne, 30 Royal Parade, Parkville, Melbourne, VIC, 3052, Australia

(2)
School of Psychology and Public Health, La Trobe University, Bundoora, VIC, 3086, Australia

 



 

Andrea Gogos




Abstract

Epidemiological and clinical evidence suggests estrogen plays a role in the development and severity of schizophrenia, and a growing body of literature indicates estrogen therapy is a feasible treatment option. Current pharmacological treatments for schizophrenia primarily address the positive symptoms and fail to adequately address the cognitive deficits; thus, novel treatments require exploration. The sex steroid hormone 17β-estradiol has been extensively studied as a treatment for schizophrenia, and selective estrogen receptor modulators (SERMs) have been more recently investigated as other potential candidates. This chapter aims to critically analyse the current evidence for the clinical applicability of 17β-estradiol and the SERM raloxifene for the treatment of schizophrenia, with particular emphasis on treating cognitive symptoms.


Keywords
SchizophreniaCognitionEstrogenEstradiolSERMsRaloxifenePositive symptomsPsychoneuroendocrinologyInformation processing



Introduction


Estrogen is a potent gonadal steroid that can have dynamic effects in the brain. A rich literature encompassing both preclinical and clinical studies describes the effect that estrogen can exert on cognition, mood, and behaviour. The past two decades have seen an increased interest in the role of estrogen in the pathophysiology and treatment of schizophrenia [13]. This chapter aims to critically analyse the current evidence for the utility of 17β-estradiol and its analogues as a form of therapy for schizophrenia, with particular focus on the feasibility of the treatment for the cognitive deficits associated with the disorder.


Schizophrenia


Schizophrenia is a complex neuropsychiatric disorder that will affect approximately seven to eight individuals per 1,000 during their lifetime [4]. The disorder is characterised by three broad categories of symptoms: positive, negative, and cognitive symptoms [5]. The positive and negative symptoms are often considered the most debilitating; however, cognitive deficits are the best predictor of functional outcome [6, 7]. Current antipsychotic drugs are not suitable for the effective treatment of the cognitive dysfunctions associated with schizophrenia; thus, novel treatments require exploration [8]. It has previously been theorised that second-generation antipsychotics can provide some cognitive benefit in patients [9]. However, more recent research suggests observed outcomes could be attributed to poor study design, practise effects, and inappropriate doses of medication [10].


Cognitive Underperformance in Schizophrenia


Cognition is a broad term referring to the mental processes related to acquiring knowledge and understanding [11]; measurable areas of cognition can include learning, processing speed, memory, and reasoning. In this chapter we focus on information processing and learning and memory. It is well established that individuals with schizophrenia suffer from working memory problems [12]. A meta-analysis by Forbes et al. in 2009 [12] concluded there are large deficits in all domains of working memory (central executive, visuospatial, phonological) in schizophrenic patients compared with healthy controls.

Cognitive and intellectual underperformance has been consistently identified as a risk factor for schizophrenia. Decline in cognitive ability precedes the onset of clinical symptoms by nearly a decade [13]. A meta-analysis of neurocognitive function has found schizophrenia patients perform on average 1.5–2.5 standard deviations below the norm on neurocognitive tests of attention, motor performance, memory, and general intelligence [10]. Importantly, even when the positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia are in remission, cognitive deficits remain [14]. This demonstrates the robustness of the cognitive dysfunctions associated with the disorder, in addition to the limited capability of current pharmacological treatments to improve function [15].


Gender Differences in Schizophrenia


The onset of schizophrenia generally occurs in late adolescence or young adulthood [16]. Examining gender differences in schizophrenia has found an earlier onset of approximately 4–6 years in males compared with females. Further, females also have a second peak of incidence at 45–50 years of age, suggested to be a period of low hormone levels due to menopause [16]. Course of illness, severity of symptoms, and response to antipsychotic medication differ between sexes, with females having a better outcome than males [1719]. Presentation of the illness is also considered to differ, with men affected by more negative symptoms and women suffering more affective symptoms [18]. Likewise, neuroanatomy is considered dissimilar between men and women with schizophrenia; males are thought to have more brain structural abnormalities than females, including enlarged ventricles and decreased temporal lobe volume [18].

Sex hormones are postulated to be a prominent factor in the dissimilar presentation of illness between men and women. Symptom severity in women with schizophrenia and risk of relapse increases during the postpartum period, after menopause, and during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle, all times of reduced hormone levels [2023]. In a 1959 study by Dalton and colleagues [24], it was reported that of their sample of 276 women admitted to psychiatric hospitals, 46% were admitted during or immediately before menstruation, a period of low plasma estrogen. More recent research further corroborates a negative correlation between estrogen levels and psychosis [25, 26]. A relationship has also been discovered between earlier puberty and later onset of schizophrenia [27]. Additionally, it has been theorised that improved pharmacological response with lower doses of typical antipsychotics in women of child-bearing age, compared with postmenopausal women, is due to the enhancing effect of estrogen via its antidopaminergic properties [18].


The Role of Estrogens in Schizophrenia


Estrogen can exert potent effects in numerous regions of the brain, consequently affecting mood, cognition, and behaviour [28, 29]. Physicians first noticed the benefits of hormones for psychiatric illness and menopausal symptoms over 100 years ago [30]. In the past 50 years, research into estrogen therapy has markedly advanced. Its medical utility has been explored in breast [31] and prostate cancer [32], and osteoporosis [33]. Moreover, researchers continue to study the use of estrogens for the delay or alleviation of neurodegenerative diseases, and treatment of psychiatric disorders including treatment-resistant depression [34], perimenopausal and postpartum depression [35, 36], bipolar affective disorder [37], and schizophrenia [2].

Evidence suggests that steroidal hormones, such as estrogen, exert their effects over the entire lifetime, protecting the brain from certain insults [38]. Accumulating evidence has led to the hypothesis that reoccurring hormone influxes in women serve as a protective factor in the initial development of schizophrenia [39]. The hypothesised role of estrogen in schizophrenia is not a recent theory. In 1961, Diczfalusy and Lauritzen [40] reviewed studies measuring estrogen concentration in the blood and urine of women with schizophrenia [40, 41]. In seven of the eight studies examined, low estrogen levels were detected [40]. This has contributed to the hypoestrogenism hypothesis, which posits that low 17β-estradiol in schizophrenia is either a trigger or initial vulnerability leading to development of the mental illness, or conversely, an outcome of the disease itself [41]. While these studies are over 70 years old, admittedly used small sample sizes, and applied laboratory methods now considered outdated, the research is of significance due to its occurrence during the pre-antipsychotic era [41]. The current use of antipsychotic treatment in women with schizophrenia, particularly typical antipsychotics, affords difficulty in reliably assessing hormone levels due to hyperprolactinemia [42]. A review of research into antipsychotic-induced hyperprolactinemia in schizophrenia patients found 67% of the sample of women had abnormal levels of prolactin [42]. Elevated prolactin levels occur when antipsychotics block dopamine D2 receptors on the anterior pituitary gland [42], and consequently can suppress gonadal function.

Despite the potential confounding effect of antipsychotics, research continues to demonstrate that low estrogen plasma concentration correlates with an increased risk of symptoms of schizophrenia [41]. For example, in pre-menopausal hospital patients with schizophrenia, Bergemann et al. [43] found a significant increase in admissions during the 3 days prior to and following the first day of menses. Further, in a more recent study Bergemann et al. [44] assessed plasma 17β-estradiol concentration in schizophrenia patients, while also controlling for antipsychotic treatment; hypoestrogenism occurred in approximately 60% of their sample. In addition, there was a significant difference in prolactin levels between the patients in the typical and atypical antipsychotic groups, however, no difference between the two atypical antipsychotic groups (clozapine and olanzapine). Ultimately, Bergemann et al. concluded that low serum levels of 17β-estradiol exist independent of antipsychotic-induced elevation of prolactin levels [44].

Gender differences in schizophrenia, and comparison of endocrinological function in schizophrenic women compared to the healthy population, provides a case for the estrogen hypothesis. Evidence including lower baseline levels of circulating estrogen, amenorrhea, [45], the association between earlier onset of puberty and later onset of the disorder [27], and superior response to antipsychotic medication in women compared to men, suggests estrogen can exert a protective effect in schizophrenia, and consequently may serve as an appropriate form of treatment. Molecular findings further strengthen the clinical observations and evidence for the estrogen hypothesis [46]. Most notably, Weickert et al. [47] discovered that estrogen receptors are altered in the brains of individuals with schizophrenia, consequently affecting their ability to respond to endogenous estrogen. Clinical and molecular findings over the past 50 years provide a substantial argument for the use of estrogen therapy in women with schizophrenia; however, the most appropriate estrogenic compound for long-term use is yet to be determined.


17β-Estradiol


There exist numerous forms of endogenous estrogen; however, 17β-estradiol is considered the most potent form. Although it is often considered the primary ‘female sex hormone’, it is present in both sexes [48]. While 17β-estradiol is predominantly produced in the ovaries to regulate the menstrual cycle in females, it is also created by non-endocrine tissues, including fat, breast, and neural tissues [49]. It is important to note that reference to estrogen treatment, particularly in early research, can broadly refer to numerous estrogenic compounds including estrone, diethylstilbestrol, equilin, 17β-estradiol, and ethinylestradiol. From here on, this chapter will specifically focus on the estrogen 17β-estradiol, unless otherwise stated.


The Effect of 17β-Estradiol on Cognition in Clinical Studies


A considerable volume of literature has been published on the facilitative effect of 17β-estradiol on cognitive performance. Researchers identified a connection between ovarian hormones and cognition after discovering that fluctuating levels across stages of the human menstrual cycle were accompanied by changes in cognitive performance [50]. This has contributed to the hypothesis that estrogen maintains a certain level of cognitive function in women [51]. This theory has been substantiated in postmenopausal women receiving hormone replacement therapy [52], and in preclinical studies using ovariectomised (OVX) rodents and 17β-estradiol replacement [53]. Clinical research outcomes, however, are inconsistent and results vary widely dependent on sample characteristics. Collectively, current evidence indicates that 17β-estradiol has the ability to facilitate cognition; however, this outcome is dependent on factors including treatment dose [54], cognitive task and brain region [55], sex, endogenous levels of hormones [56], treatment window [51], and mental and physical health [57].

General consensus regarding endogenous 17β-estradiol in naturally cycling women is that verbal and fine motor skill task performance improves when estrogen levels are elevated, while decreased levels assist in spatial task performance [51]. For example, Hampson [61] found verbal fluency and articulation was improved in young women during the mid-luteal (high estrogen) phase of the menstrual cycle. Sundström et al. [58] however, have concluded in their meta-analysis that there is no consistent pattern found among the existing studies of verbal ability and menstrual cycle [6266]. It is important to note, however, that while the collective literature is inconsistent for verbal memory and menstrual cycle, exogenous estrogen has been found to benefit verbal memory [60].

As Sundström et al. [58] identify, many of the studies reviewed in their meta-analysis have poor design and low power. Additionally, days of the menstrual cycle during which participants were tested varied between studies, which can result in diverse hormone levels and dissimilar outcomes between research findings, despite testing within the same menstrual period. Although researchers can examine the relationship between certain cognitive abilities and menstrual phases, it is important to consider the effects of individual hormones within the menstrual phases. Studies demonstrating a proclivity for enhanced verbal ability during the mid-luteal phase often attribute the effect to estrogen; however, this period involves a rise in both 17β-estradiol and progesterone. Only in recent years have researchers taken this factor into consideration. For example, Maki et al. [64] assayed hormones to determine whether a correlation exists between levels of 17β-estradiol, progesterone, and cognitive measures. In a sample of young women, they [64] found verbal scores were positively associated with 17β-estradiol levels, while spatial ability scores were negatively related. However, correlations between progesterone and cognition were not statistically significant. While it is possible to investigate relationships between endogenous hormones and cognitive tasks in humans, it is difficult to account for potential hormonal interactions. Fortunately, preclinical research investigating exogenous 17β-estradiol and cognition can further elucidate the outcomes seen in clinical studies.


Estrogen Therapy in the Clinical Population


Generally, there is no clinical necessity for healthy women of childbearing age to be treated with 17β-estradiol. Although the effects of estrogen-based contraceptives in healthy young women have been investigated [67, 68], the contraceptive formulations do not include 17β-estradiol, but rather synthetic derivatives (e.g., ethinylestradiol). Therefore, research examining the effect of 17β-estradiol treatment has primarily been concerned with neurodegenerative diseases [69], psychiatric disorders [2], and the postmenopausal population [70]. Perhaps the most notable study in this area of research is the Women’s Health Initiative (WHI) memory study, a large double-blind, randomised placebo-controlled trial, which investigated cognitive function in a postmenopausal sample [71]. Following cognitive testing of 1,416 women receiving hormone replacement therapy, researchers found there was no beneficial effect of estrogen on cognition [71]. Importantly, participants partaking in the WHI study were treated with conjugated equine estrogens (CEE). Conjugated estrogens primarily contain estrone and equilin, with lesser quantities of 17β-estradiol [56]. This is important to note, as 17β-estradiol and the main component of CEE, estrone, have shown opposing effects on cognition in preclinical research [56]. Estrone has been found to impair spatial working memory [72], while 17β-estradiol has the opposite effect [73]. The discrepancy between CEE and 17β-estradiol is often not outlined in research administering CEE as estrogen therapy, which has consequently contributed to the incorrect conclusion that 17β-estradiol is not beneficial, or is even harmful, for cognitive function in postmenopausal women. Research concerning estrogen treatment and CEE in the postmenopausal population will not be detailed further in this chapter; however, see Fischer et al. [28] for an overview of hormone therapy relevant to cognition in postmenopausal women. Similarly, Gogos et al. [68] recently reviewed literature concerning ethinylestradiol-based oral contraceptives and their effect on cognition in pre-menopausal women.


Estrogen Therapy in Schizophrenia


A growing body of literature provides evidence that estrogen treatment in conjunction with antipsychotics is beneficial for treating the positive symptoms of schizophrenia [2, 74, 75]. An initial pilot study by Kulkarni et al. [76] discovered that the 17β-estradiol derivative, ethinylestradiol, taken orally daily for 8 weeks, significantly improved positive symptoms compared to the antipsychotic-only group. Later, trialling a transdermal method of administration, the same investigators determined that schizophrenia patients receiving adjunctive 17β-estradiol had significant improvements in positive symptoms compared to the placebo group [75].

Although research has found low levels of endogenous estrogen correlate with more severe negative symptoms [77], few studies thus far have demonstrated significant changes in negative symptoms following 17β-estradiol treatment. Conversely, the beneficial effect of estrogen treatment for the positive symptoms of schizophrenia has been replicated [2, 7476, 78, 79]. However, there have also been instances of inability to replicate. For example, Bergemann et al. [80] failed to demonstrate the beneficial effect of 17β-estradiol on positive or negative symptoms in their placebo-controlled double-blind study. Similarly, Lindamer et al. [81], using a cross-sectional sample, found no effect of estrogen on positive symptomatology in postmenopausal women with schizophrenia, however, negative symptoms were improved. Importantly, Bergemann et al. [80] used a combined 17β-estradiol and progestin oral treatment, with different compounds and doses dependent on the phase of menstrual cycle. Lindamer et al. [81] did not administer pharmacological intervention, but rather used a cross-sectional sample of women with schizophrenia who had received hormone replacement therapy for at least 1 year, and women with schizophrenia receiving no hormone replacement therapy.

While research into 17β-estradiol therapy thus far suggests that it aids the positive symptoms of schizophrenia [2, 76, 78], the effect on cognitive symptoms remains less clear. Examining endogenous estrogen, researchers have determined a relationship between 17β-estradiol and performance in certain cognitive tasks in women with schizophrenia. A study by Hoff et al. [82] found that improvements in verbal memory, perceptual motor speed, and spatial memory were positively correlated with 17β-estradiol levels. Ko and colleagues [77] determined a similar trend; they divided schizophrenia patients into two groups using normal serum 17β-estradiol reference ranges during the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle. Researchers found that women with low baseline levels of estradiol had diminished performance in verbal memory and executive function, compared to the group of women with higher baseline levels of 17β-estradiol [77].

Clinical research specifically concerning the influence of exogenous 17β-estradiol on cognition in schizophrenia patients is limited. Bergemann and colleagues [83] found that oral 17β-estradiol and adjunctive antipsychotic treatment for women with schizophrenia improved comprehension of metaphoric speech, but had no effect on verbal ability. Alternatively, using a transdermal method of administration of 17β-estradiol, Kulkarni et al. [2] found there were no significant differences between or within groups in cognitive domains including memory, language, constructional skills, and attention. Evidently, the effect of estrogen therapy on cognition differs between trials and within populations. Conflicting outcomes in the literature are likely due to a variety of inconsistent factors including dissimilar measures, variable treatment duration, additional pharmacotherapy, baseline endogenous hormone levels, method of treatment administration, and pharmacological and pharmacokinetic variations in estrogen.


The Effect of 17β-Estradiol on Cognition in Preclinical Studies


The beneficial effect of 17β-estradiol on cognition has been consistently replicated in animal studies. It should be noted that this section of the chapter only refers to studies of 17β-estradiol in rat cognition; see Gibbs [84] for research relevant to mice and non-human primates. In rats (Table 26.1), the beneficial effect of estrogen has been seen in learning and memory including spatial working, recognition, and reference memory domains [53, 73, 131, 135]. Luine et al. [73] examined spatial memory in OVX rats in an eight-arm radial maze; 3 days of 17β-estradiol treatment via subcutaneous implant did not enhance memory performance compared to the untreated OVX rats; however, 12 days of treatment significantly improved memory. Using the novel-object and placement-recognition paradigm, Luine et al. [53] demonstrated that 17β-estradiol treatment enhanced visual and place memory in OVX rats compared to controls. In addition, estrogen treatment enhanced memory when given prior to or immediately after the sample trial, but not, however, when administered 2 h later. Thus, it is theorised that 17β-estradiol treatment affects memory encoding or consolidation, rather than retrieval. Interestingly, authors also determined that compared to novel-object recognition, a different dose of 17β-estradiol for novel-place recognition was necessary to see a significant effect [53].


Table 26.1
Research investigating the effect of 17β-estradiol treatment on cognition in ovariectomised (OVX) female rats
























































































































































































































































































































































































































   
17β-estradiol
   

Cognitive domain

Test

Method

Dose

Effects

Reference

Attention and impulsivity

5-choice serial reaction time task

Injection

Chronic


Barnes et al. [193]
   
Implant

Chronic

↑ Treatment at 17 months

∅ Treatment at 12 months

Bohacek and Daniel [90]

Learning and memory (classical and operant conditioning)

Active avoidance

Injection

Acute

↑ High dose

↓ Low and moderate dose

Diaz-Veliz et al. [96]
   
Injection

Chronic


Horvath et al. [116]
   
Implant

Chronic


Singh et al. [137]
 
Fear conditioning

Injection

Chronic

↑ Contextual conditioning

∅ Cued fear conditioning

Barha et al. [87]
   
Injection

Chronic

↑ Context discrimination

∅ Acquisition or extinction

Hoffman et al. [114]
 
Inhibitory avoidance

Injection

Acute


Rhodes and Frye [132]
   
Injection

Acute

↑ Post-training

∅ 1–3 hours post-training

Rhodes and Frye [131]
   
Injection

Chronic


Horvath et al. [116]
   
Injection

Chronic


Frye and Rhodes [102]
   
Implant

Chronic


Frye and Rhodes [102]

Recognition memory

Novel object recognition

Injection

Acute

↑ Immediately post-training

∅ 1 hour post-training

Walf et al. [146]
   
Injection

Acute

↑ Moderate dose

∅ Low and high doses

Inagaki et al. [54]
   
Injection

Acute

↑ Prior to training

↑ Immediately post-training

∅ 2 hours post-training

Luine et al. [53]
   
Injection

Acute


Jacome et al. [117]

Spatial recognition memory

Y-maze

Injection

Acute

↑ Low dose

∅ Moderate or high doses

Hawley et al. [113]
 
Novel object place

Injection

Acute

↑ Moderate doses

∅ Low and high doses

Inagaki et al. [54]
   
Injection

Acute

↑ Prior to training

↑ Immediately post-training

∅ 2 hours post-training

Luine et al. [53]
   
Injection

Acute

↑ High dose

∅ Low dose

McLaughlin et al. [127]
   
Injection

Acute

↑ Immediately post-training

∅ 1.5 hour post-training

Frye et al. [103]
   
Injection

Acute


Jacome et al. [117]
   
Implant

Chronic


Walf et al. [147]

Spatial learning and memory

Barnes maze

Injection

Chronic


Ping et al. [130]
 
Morris water maze

Injection

Acute


Chesler and Juraska [91], McLaughlin et al. [127]
   
Injection

Acute


Sandstrom and Williams [135], Rhodes and Frye [132], Markham et al. [125]
   
Injection

Acute

↑ Low dose

∅ High dose

McLaughlin et al. [127]
   
Injection

Acute

↓ During acquisition

Frick et al. [101]
   
Injection

Chronic


El-Bakri et al. [97], Feng et al. [100], Bimonte-Nelson et al. [89]
   
Implant

Chronic


Bimonte-Nelson et al. [89], Markham et al. [125]
   
Implant

Chronic

↓ During acquisition

Daniel and Lee [93]
   
Implant

Chronic


Singh et al. [137]
   
Implant

Chronic

↑ Young and middle-aged

∅ Older

Talboom et al. [139]
   
Implant

Chronic

↑ Middle-aged and older

∅ Young

Kiss et al. [118]
   
Orally

Chronic


Liu et al. [122]
   
Orally

Chronic

↑ Continuous treatment

∅ Cycling treatment

Lowry et al. [123]
   
Orally

Chronic


Wu et al. [152]
 
T-maze

Implant

Chronic


Gibbs [105]
   
Implant

Chronic

↑ Treatment 3 months post-OVX

∅ Treatment 10 months post-OVX

Gibbs [106]
   
Mini-osmotic pump

Chronic


Hammond et al. [112]
 
Open-field tower maze

Injections

Chronic

↑ Cycling treatment

∅ Continuous treatment

Lipatova et al. [121]
   
Implant

Chronic


Lipatova and Toufexis [120]
 
Plus maze

Injection

Acute

↑ Place learning

↓ Response learning

Korol and Kolo [119]

Spatial working memory

Delayed spatial alternation

Implant

Chronic


Wang et al. [148, 149]
 
Radial arm maze

Injection

Chronic

↓ Spatial working-reference memory, cued win-stay, conditioned place preference

∅ Delayed win-shift task

Galea et al. [104]
   
Injection

Chronic

↑ Low dose

↓ High doses

Holmes et al. [115]
   
Implant

Chronic


Daniel et al. [92]
   
Implant

Chronic

↑ Moderate dose

∅ Low dose

Bimonte and Denenberg [88]
   
Implant

Chronic

↑ Immediately post-OVX

∅ Five months post-OVX

Daniel et al. [94]
   
Implant

Chronic

↑ Place learning

↓ Response learning

Davis et al. [95]
   
Implant

Chronic

↑ Working memory

∅ Reference memory

Fader et al. [99], Luine et al. [73], Gibbs and Johnson [108]
   
Implant

Chronic


Luine and Rodriguez [124]
 
Y-maze

Injection

Acute


McLaughlin et al. [127]
   
Injection

Acute


Velásquez-Zamora et al. [145]

Working memory

Non-spatial delayed alternation T-maze

Injection

Chronic

↑ Low dose, short delay

↓ High doses, long delay

Wide et al. [150]


Note ↓ impaired performance, ↑ facilitated performance, ∅ no effect or difference compared to control, Chronic >3 days treatment, OVX ovariectomy

Dose-dependent effects of 17β-estradiol have also been demonstrated in reference memory [55]. In their 2010 review, Barha and Galea concluded that high levels of 17β-estradiol can impede working and reference memory, whereas low levels of 17β-estradiol have no significant effect on reference memory, however can facilitate working memory [55]. Similarly, contextual fear conditioning can be facilitated by a low dose of 17β-estradiol, however, a high dose can impair [55, 115]. This demonstrates the capacity of 17β-estradiol to differentially affect forms of memory, all of which are hippocampus-dependent. Collectively, the literature indicates an inverted U-shaped dose–response curve [54]; lower and higher doses of 17β-estradiol can often inhibit or impair cognition [56].

Numerous studies have shown that behavioural tasks employing the hippocampus can be altered by 17β-estradiol [88, 92, 99, 102, 107, 112]; however, fewer have investigated cortical-dependent tasks [29]. Wide et al. [150] examined the effect of 17β-estradiol in OVX rats in the non-spatial delayed alternation task, mediated by the integrity of the prefrontal cortex. A lower dose of 17β-estradiol was most effective for facilitation of non-spatial working memory; subjects receiving a high dose made significantly more errors compared to the controls, demonstrating that a task considered primarily prefrontal cortical-dependent can also be affected by 17β-estradiol [150].


Pharmacological Models of Schizophrenia


Pharmacologically disrupted prepulse inhibition is frequently used to model psychosis-like symptoms in rodents. Prepulse inhibition, a measure of sensory gating, is considered to represent the interface of psychosis and cognition [156, 157]. In healthy subjects, the impact of a startle-inducing acoustic stimulus (a pulse) is successfully attenuated by a preceding stimulus (a prepulse), and consequently the magnitude of startle response is reduced. However, individuals with schizophrenia do not experience the same level of filtering [158]. Administering certain drug treatments in animals allows for observation of cognitive-behavioural and neurochemical changes similar to those seen in schizophrenia patients [157]. In studies of rat prepulse inhibition, 17β-estradiol can attenuate disruptions induced by the serotonin-1A receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT, the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801, and the dopamine D1/2 receptor agonist, apomorphine [109111, 142, 159].

There are a number of cross-species paradigms used to measure information processing; another method of measuring auditory sensory gating is the P50 event-related potential (ERP) suppression paradigm, measured using electroencephalography. Thwaites et al. [141] tested the effect of 17β-estradiol on sensory gating in OVX and intact rats. Similarly to the prepulse inhibition studies of Gogos et al. [111], Thwaites et al. induced deficits by administering dopaminergic and glutamatergic drugs. Subjects were injected acutely with apomorphine, amphetamine, and phencyclidine. Chronic estrogen treatment via subcutaneous implant successfully prevented apomorphine-induced sensory gating disruption in OVX rats, but had no effect on amphetamine or phencyclidine.

Latent inhibition is another cognitive-behavioural assay used to assess the neurobiological underpinning of schizophrenia. Disrupted latent inhibition reflects a deficit in selective attention, whereby the subject loses the ability to ignore an irrelevant stimulus. Similar to prepulse inhibition and P50 ERP, latent inhibition gauges the animal’s ability to filter out unnecessary information. Arad and Weiner [86] tested the antipsychotic effect of 17β-estradiol in drug-induced disruption of latent inhibition. Intact female rats received pre-treatment of estradiol prior to acute amphetamine administration, and disruption of latent inhibition was successfully reversed. Similarly, in OVX rats treated with MK-801, 17β-estradiol pre-treatment successfully reversed MK-801-induced latent inhibition persistence. Intriguingly, a low 17β-estradiol dose has been found to disrupt latent inhibition in both OVX and intact rats [85, 128]. It is theorised that a high dose of 17β-estradiol can exert an antipsychotic effect, while low doses exert a pro-psychotic effect [86, 128].

Preclinical research on 17β-estradiol and memory relevant to models of schizophrenia has primarily focused on recognition memory [134, 160]. Using an acute dose of the NMDA receptor antagonist phencyclidine in intact female rats, Sutcliffe et al. [160] found 17β-estradiol treatment attenuated drug-induced memory deficits in the novel-object recognition task. Using chronic 17β-estradiol and sub-chronic phencyclidine treatment, Roseman et al. [134] demonstrated comparable results to Sutcliffe et al., however, by using OVX instead of intact rats; 17β-estradiol alleviated deficits in recognition memory when administered either before or after phencyclidine.

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Oct 20, 2017 | Posted by in PSYCHIATRY | Comments Off on The Effect of 17β-Estradiol and Its Analogues on Cognition in Preclinical and Clinical Research: Relevance to Schizophrenia

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access