Communication disorders

11 Communication disorders






Introduction


The spoken language used between one human and another is, without a doubt, one of the most important and complex interactive tools that we use. That said, we must remember that non-verbal communication is also widely used by us through gesture, body posture and facial expression. Patients with speech problems gave early researchers the first clues about the involvement of the brain in language. It was the Ancient Greeks who noticed that brain damage could cause language function to be impaired (aphasia). They noticed that if the brain was damaged by a physical trauma (e.g. a head injury in battle) then the speech function of the individual concerned could be affected.


Communication difficulties are common in people with neurological conditions and are often encountered in those with a head injury, stroke and progressive neurological conditions such as Parkinson’s disease and dementia, to name but a few. In patient-centred health care, good communication between patient and health professional is essential, e.g. to establish initial rapport with a patient and their family, obtain information about a patient’s health status, accurately assess a patient’s abilities and difficulties, negotiate treatment goals, clarify information about interventions or monitor pain. For these reasons, it is essential that health professionals have a sound understanding of the speech and language disorders they are most likely to come across.


The aim of this chapter is to explain the concept of language disorders (known as aphasia) and speech disorders (such as dysarthria and apraxia of speech), explore their underlying neural mechanisms and discuss the impact that speech and language disorders may have on a person’s ability to communicate. There are many different types and subtleties in speech and language disorders, which is unsurprising given the complexity and scope of human communication. This chapter is designed to give you an introduction to this topic to enable you to develop your understanding of some of the most common issues in communication disorders, and assist you in problem solving when working with people with speech and language impairments.



An overview of speech and language processes


What is language? It is a complex form of communication involving written or spoken words used to symbolise objects and convey ideas. Reading, writing, drawing, gesturing, making eye contact and facial expression, speaking and listening, adjusting one’s tone of voice, are all part of our ‘toolkit’ for communication. It involves ‘expression’ and ‘comprehension’, with each aspect being related to a specific neural network – as we shall see later on in this chapter.


Before we examine impairments in speech and language, it may be useful to consider what is involved in normal communication. Think for a moment about a situation where you wish to express yourself, e.g. you have a question about what you have just read. Firstly, you need to formulate an idea in your mind, using knowledge and understanding you have stored already. People with cognitive impairments may have difficulty at this stage. Next, you need to formulate your question, and find the right words with the right meanings and place these in the appropriate logical order using the correct grammatical coding (e.g. correct tense). Aphasia is the collection of impairments at this level of communication. You must then select the correct motor program in order to articulate your actual question and use your articulatory muscles (for example your tongue and lips) to speak. Problems at these final levels are known as apraxia of speech and dysarthria respectively. Figure 11.1 gives an overview of the various functions required for communicating an idea, and examples of common disorders where these functions may be found to be impaired.



In this chapter we are going to go into more detail about aphasia, apraxia of speech and dysarthria. You may hear aphasia being referred to as dysphasia. While aphasia literally means total loss of language, dysphasia means partial loss of language. However, nowadays the two terms are used interchangeably to indicate a damaged language system. It is important to understand that aphasia is a language problem; a higher order communication impairment. Our society tends to consider the ability to communicate through speech and writing to be signs of intelligence. However, while language difficulties that arise as a result of aphasia may hinder a person’s understanding or expressing themselves in the spoken and/or written medium, it does not affect their ability to think, feel, remember and plan, and, therefore, intelligence is not considered to be affected by aphasia.


Dysarthria or apraxia of speech are motor speech problems. Speech in people with dysarthria often sounds slow and laboured, with consonants often having a ‘slurred’ quality and vowels may sound distorted. Intonation and voice quality may also sound ‘abnormal’. In contrast to those with higher order communication impairments, however, people with dysarthria have no problems with comprehension and no problems thinking of what they want to say, selecting the correct words, sequencing them in the correct order or selecting the right motor programme (which is a problem for people with apraxia of speech). The problem here is the tone and coordination of the muscles involved in speaking, for example, tongue, lips, soft palate, vocal folds and respiration system. Because of the slurred aspect of this motor speech disorder, many people with dysarthria find that some members of the public treat them as if they have a learning disability or assume that they have been drinking alcohol. Although aphasia often coexists with motor speech problems, it is important for health-care professionals to be aware of this distinction to avoid misinterpretation and poor communication.



Aphasia: brain–behaviour relationship


Perhaps one of the single most important discoveries in speech and language research was made by the French neurologist, Marc Dax. In 1836, at a conference in Montpellier, Dax described a group of patients who had difficulties speaking and he further reported that all of the patients had left-sided brain damage. Although Dax’s paper on left hemispheric dominance for spoken communication was written and published in 1936, a year before his death, the left-sided localisation of speech and language is often attributed to Paul Broca, who we shall discuss shortly.


Although the language centres are located in the left hemisphere in approximately 95% of the population, it was thought that if damage to the brain occurs within the first 3 years of life, language acquisition follows normal development after an initial delay, with some of the function being transferred to the right hemisphere. This transfer, however, may be at the expense of other non-verbal skills. Up to the age of 10 years, language ability is often re-established following brain damage, with permanent impairment often observed if damage occurs beyond the early teens. There is an unconfirmed hypothesis that cerebral plasticity ceases by the age of 10 as a result of the establishment of cerebral dominance of language function. However, research indicates that even for some children under 10 years of age who have had brain damage and in whom language appears to have fully recovered, subtle but persistent deficits (e.g. literacy problems), still persist. Findings such as these suggest that the brain regions involved in language comprehension and expression are permanently assigned before adolescence. So the majority of patients who experience brain damage to the left hemisphere may experience language disorders, in fact it is thought that one-third of patients who have a stroke experience aphasia. We will discuss the intricacies of aphasia a little later, but first let’s go back to Broca’s work.



Broca’s area


As mentioned earlier, the French neuroanatomist/anthropologist Paul Broca is often credited with the localisation of language function, specifically the motor aspects of speech production, in the left hemisphere of the brain. This is despite the fact that Marc Dax had published a paper on left hemispheric dominance for language some 25 years before Broca. There is actually little doubt that Broca was aware of the work of Dax, but he steadfastly refused to acknowledge the original theoretical contribution – ‘I do not like dealing with the questions of priority concerning myself. That is the reason why I did not mention the name of Dax in my paper’. Not only did he fail to acknowledge Dax, he actually claimed to be the first to discuss the theory of left hemispheric dominance! An interesting article was written by the authors Cubelli and Montagna (1994) who state that ‘the weight of evidence reported … . suggests that the theory of the left hemisphere dominance for speech must be attributed equally to Dax and Broca, and henceforth should be called the theory of Dax-Broca’. Let us leave this debate behind and examine the work of Broca a little more closely.


Broca (1861) examined a man, who was able to understand what was said to him but was unable to pronounce any words other than ‘tan’ over and over again. Although he was referred to as ‘Tan’, the unfortunate individual was a M. Leborgne who died a mere 6 days after examination. His death gave Broca the chance to examine the brain which revealed that the area damaged was a small region towards the front left hand side of brain – a region that became known as Broca’s area (Fig. 11.2).



Broca’s area is often referred to as the motor-speech area and it is located adjacent to the precentral gyrus of the motor cortex in the frontal lobes (see Fig. 11.2). This area controls the movements required for articulation, facial expression and phonation. Broca’s patients generally had good overall comprehension but their speech was often limited in their vocabulary and grammar which contained inaccurate pronunciation of words or parts of words which often made their speech unintelligible. More about the clinical presentation of people with Broca’s aphasia can be found below.



Wernicke’s area


Karl Wernicke, a Prussian physician, discovered a different kind of speech and language problem a few years later, which uncovered damage to a completely different region of the brain – unsurprisingly thereafter called Wernicke’s area. Wernicke’s patients generally possessed good articulation, but their speech often didn’t make sense, with words jumbled together in an incoherent sequence. They frequently used ‘made-up’ words with no meaning (neologisms).


Wernicke’s area, which includes the auditory comprehension centre, lies in the posterior superior temporal lobe near the primary auditory cortex at the junction of the parietal, temporal and occipital lobes (see Fig 11.2). Wernicke’s area plays a critical role in understanding both spoken and written messages (See Clinical application box 11.1), as well as being responsible for formulating coherent speech patterns. These ‘commands’ formulated in Wernicke’s area are transferred via a fibre tract, the arcuate fasciculus, to Broca’s area. Wernicke’s receives input from both the visual cortex in occipital lobe (an important pathway in reading, comprehension and describing objects seen) and the auditory cortex (essential for understanding spoken words).



Wernicke suggested that this joining of Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas by the arcuate fasciculus results in the formation of a complex network, rather than language simply being produced by two independent language centres. He further proposed that damage to this connection would lead to conduction aphasia, which will be discussed later on. Damage to specific brain regions can lead to selective language disturbances and these clinical presentations will be discussed in more detail in the following sections.



Aphasia: clinical presentation


As mentioned earlier, communication is a very complex human skill. Damage to the language component of communication, i.e. aphasia, can therefore affect some or all modalities of language processing: expression and comprehension of speech, reading and writing, gesture and the use of language (i.e. pragmatics). The severity can also vary between individuals, so someone may have occasional problems thinking of a word (which means that there will be some pauses or some errors when they are speaking to you), whereas other people may be more severely affected and unable to put their ideas and intentions into spoken and/or written language, having no intelligible communication. As aphasia also affects the understanding of language it can affect the ability of a person to understand even simple sentences or single words. Aphasia can also affect the ability to understand and use various methods of communication other than speech, for example, gesture. And the severity of aphasia can also fluctuate from one day to the next where an individual may have many word-finding problems one day and on another they may speak much more fluently.


So with such an array of complexities how is it possible to describe your patient’s type of aphasia? This is not a new problem. As more information was discovered over the years about the various complexities of language, researchers devised a large number of ways of describing and classifying the different types of aphasia. However, as we mentioned above it is important to remember that the aphasic population is heterogeneous in nature and any number and variations of the characteristics of aphasia can affect individuals. Therefore, it is important for health professionals to understand that aphasia is variable and different for each individual. In saying that, it is very useful to be able to discuss a patient with colleagues, e.g. during a multidisciplinary team (MDT) meeting, in terms of the general type of aphasia a patient presents with, using terminology that is understood. Some of the ways that people with aphasia are classified include: expressive versus receptive aphasia; fluent versus non-fluent aphasia and the Boston Classification approach (who have a localisationalist viewpoint) including Broca’s, Wernicke’s and conduction aphasia. It is likely that this is the terminology that you will come across in a patient’s medical notes and, therefore, they will be described below.


You must remember though that these are only some of the ways to describe aphasia and there are often various issues in determining the exact boundaries between one type of aphasia and another.



May 25, 2016 | Posted by in NEUROLOGY | Comments Off on Communication disorders

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access