Sleep disorders

14 Sleep disorders



Normal sleep


Sleep is usually described as a series of phases characterized by changes in physiological variables, most notably the electroencephalogram (EEG), as illustrated in Figure 14.1. There is usually a typical body posture (associated with good thermoregulation); physical inactivity; more stimulation required to arouse than during wakefulness; a specific site or nest for this behaviour; and regular daily occurrence. The progression between the phases can be shown by means of a ‘hypnogram’ (Figure 14.2). There are a number of theories regarding the function of sleep (listed in Table 14.1). Evidence is accumulating for each, suggesting that sleep serves a number of functions. Sleep has been found to facilitate mathematical insight, as shown in Figure 14.3.




Table 14.1 Theories of sleep





















Conservation of energy – energy use decreases to between 5–% and 25% of waking level
Restorative – sleep allows the body to ‘mend itself’ after the ravages of the day. Anabolism/catabolism ratio increases. Increased growth hormone release
Consolidation of memory
Vestigial – sleep is the remnant of a mechanism useful at an earlier stage of evolution
Safety – superficially similar to death, sleep may discourage predators from attack, but those animals more at risk of attack sleep less
Social bonding – the clustering of humans at sleep time may be useful in keeping a social group together
To dream – the function of dreaming is not clear, but if artificially suppressed (e.g. with drugs) there is a significant increase on the cessation of suppression. This suggests that dreaming may serve an important function
Behavioural – sleep occurring because of the absence of stimulation and the lack of anything better to do
Humoral – due to accumulation of sleep-inducing substances in the brain during wakefulness

image

Figure 14.3 • Facilitation of mathematical insight by sleep.


• (a) Number Reduction Task (NRT), illustrated by an example trial. On each trial, a different string of eight digits was presented. Each string was composed of the digits ‘1’, ‘4’, and ‘9’. For each string, subjects had to determine a digit defined as the ‘final solution’ of the task trial (Fin). This could be achieved by sequentially processing the digits pairwise from left to right according to two simple rules. One, the ‘same rule’, states that the result of two identical digits is just this digit (for example, ‘1’ and ‘1’ results in ‘1’, as in response 1 here). The other rule, the ‘different rule’, states that the result of two non-identical digits is the remaining third digit of this three-digit system (for example, ‘1’ and ‘4’ results in ‘9’ as in response 2 here). After the first response, comparisons are made between the preceding result and the next digit. The seventh response indicates the final solution, to be confirmed by pressing a separate key. Not mentioned to the subjects, the strings were generated in such a way that the last three responses always mirrored the previous three responses. This implies that in each trial the second response coincided with the final solution (arrow). Subjects who gain insight into this hidden rule abruptly cut short sequential responding by pressing the solution key immediately after the second response. (b) Experimental design (main experiment). An 8-h period of nocturnal sleep, nocturnal wakefulness, or daytime wakefulness separated an initial training phase (three blocks) from later retesting (ten blocks). (c) Columns indicate percentage of subjects gaining insight into the hidden rule in the three experimental conditions of the main experiment (grey), in which subjects either slept (at night) or remained awake (at night or during daytime) between initial training and retesting, and in two supplementary conditions (hatched), where subjects were tested after nocturnal sleep or daytime wakefulness in the absence of initial training before these periods.


(Reproduced with permission from Wagner U et al 2004 Nature 427:352–355)


With less than three hours sleep in 24 hours, humans show increased irritability and a decreased attention span. Long periods of sleep deprivation result in poor concentration, deterioration in general performance, increased suggestibility and, later, hallucinations, paranoia and even seizures. The prominence of psychological effects suggests to some that sleep specifically restores brain functioning.


Normal sleep-dependent consolidation of memory is disrupted in schizophrenia. This is illustrated in Figure 14.4.




Length of time asleep


Humans spend, on average, 25% of their lives asleep. There is variation in sleep time, both in individuals during the course of development and the life cycle, and also between individuals (Figure 14.4). The individual differences within and between subjects are particularly noticeable in infancy. The mean daily sleep length in the first week of life is 16 hours (SD two hours). There is a gradual reduction in mean daily sleep length throughout infancy and middle childhood (Figure 14.5). Most infants wake at least once each night but, apart from babies who need feeding (more often those who are breastfed), they usually drop off to sleep again by themselves. Parents are not normally aware of this unless they themselves are having problems sleeping. Adults will also wake briefly during the night but often will not remember. Frequency of waking increases with age.





Classification of sleep disorders


Sleep disorders may be divided into those primarily of emotional origin and those with an organic basis. The ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR categorizations of sleep disorders are shown in Tables 14.2 and 14.3 respectively. This chapter will cover predominantly those sleep disorders with a psychiatric basis. However, sleep disorders with organic causes are important differential diagnoses and interactions with psychological factors are usual. Also, sleep disorders are often a component or complication of other psychiatric disorders.


Table 14.2 ICD-10 classification: F51 Non-organic sleep disorders















F51.0 Non-organic insomnia
F51.1 Non-organic hypersomnia
F51.2 Non-organic disorder of the sleep–wake schedule
F51.3 Sleepwalking
F51.4 Sleep terrors
F51.5 Nightmares

Table 14.3 DSM-IV-TR Sleep disorders





















Primary sleep disorders
Dyssomnias






Parasomnias




Sleep disorders related to another mental disorder


Other sleep disorders







NOS, not otherwise specified.



Insomnia


Insomnia is a disorder in which there is insufficient quantity or quality of sleep. The diagnosis may be used at any age (previously sleep disorder in childhood was classified separately). Insomnia may be transient (a history of only a few days or weeks) or chronic (an unremitting history over months or years).





Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Jul 12, 2016 | Posted by in PSYCHIATRY | Comments Off on Sleep disorders

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access