Synopsis and implications of neuroscience for neurological rehabilitation

15 Synopsis and implications of neuroscience for neurological rehabilitation





Synopsis


We started by laying the foundation of neuroscience. Following a brief synopsis of the fascinating history of neuroscience, we set out the basic structure and function of the nervous system, explored normal and abnormal changes in the nervous system across the life span, and examined the various ways in which drugs work. This was an area that could have had a whole textbook devoted to it and, indeed, there are many such books on the market. What we attempted to do was ‘cherry-pick’ the key areas that we thought would be of most interest and/or relevance.



Redundancy and plasticity


Having laid the foundation, we then moved on to explore how the brain works – or at least an exploration of our current, and still limited, understanding of how we think it works! This revealed the brain as the most complex structure in the universe; a complex, heterogeneous, integrated organ that exhibits both redundancy (i.e. spare capacity) and plasticity (i.e. the capability to change). Redundancy and plasticity are key phenomena in neuroscience and their relevance for rehabilitation cannot be underestimated. Redundancy implies that there is spare capacity that can be tapped into, and plasticity means that the brain responds to change – and thus to therapeutic input. Plasticity can go ‘haywire’, however, and the phenomenon of spasticity was used as an example of how disorganised movement may result from dysfunctional neural connections. The implications for health-care professionals are to provide timely therapeutic input of the right intensity and type, to drive neuroplastic changes that will ultimately enable long-term, functional improvement.


Interventions that involve intensive activity of a functional nature where possible, comprising an element of problem solving, currently seem to be the most likely candidates. Although there are promising lines of evidence in specific patient populations (e.g. constraint-induced movement therapy for people with some active hand movement after stroke), much more research is needed to establish a library of interventions that are effective, acceptable and meaningful to people with different levels of ability, wanting to achieve different rehabilitation goals.




Perceptuo-motor control and learning as a problem-solving activity


We started off with normal motor control and explored the role of different levels of the nervous system in the control of reflex, as well as voluntary movement. We looked at various expressions of disordered motor control, and compared and contrasted spasticity with rigidity and hypertonia, which highlighted the importance of differentiating between neural and biomechanical contributions to soft-tissue stiffness.


Next, we looked at more complex processes involved in perceptuo-motor control and introduced various theoretical models. We focused on an information processing approach, which describes successive stages of motor control from sensory input to planning, programming, execution and, finally, feedback. According to this approach, movement is coordinated by a generalised motor programme (GMP) that stores the equivalent of a ‘blueprint’ for each class of action. This template can be fine-tuned to the specific requirements of the task within the environment in which the action takes place. Although there is no consensus (yet) regarding the most convincing theoretical model of motor control, most movement scientists would agree that purposeful movement is a problem-solving activity. In the search for a solution to this problem, motor control operates at the interface between the individual with their particular characteristics, the specific task requirements, and the environmental conditions.


The implications for therapists are that they need to consider this interface in its entirety, as focusing on perfecting movement per se (e.g. gait) without training this in the environment in which the walking is to take place, and without exploring relevant task variations (e.g. sideward stepping to avoid obstacles, or holding a shopping bag while walking) is unlikely to carry over into a walking activity that is useful for the patient following discharge.

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

May 25, 2016 | Posted by in NEUROLOGY | Comments Off on Synopsis and implications of neuroscience for neurological rehabilitation

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access