Limb Apraxias and Related Disorders

Chapter 10 Limb Apraxias and Related Disorders




Apraxia is an inability to correctly perform learned skilled movements. In the limb apraxias, there is an inability to correctly execute these movements in an arm or hand owing to neurological dysfunction. Apraxia is a cognitive deficit in motor programming and results in errors of either the spatiotemporal processing of the movements or in the content of the actions. In the context of an apraxia examination, these errors can help distinguish the major types of limb apraxias.


A first step in recognizing the limb apraxias is distinguishing them from other causes of impaired movement. First of all, it is not apraxia if the impaired movements result from elementary motor deficits such as weakness, hemipareses, spasticity, ataxia, or extrapyramidal disturbances. Second, apraxia is distinguishable from impaired movements due to primary sensory deficits, hemispatial neglect, spatial or object agnosia, or other complex visual disorder. Third, apraxia is distinct from abnormal movements or postures such as tremor, myoclonus, choreoathetosis, or dystonic posturing. Finally, it is not apraxia if the impaired movements result from other cognitive disorders involving attention, memory, language comprehension, or executive functions (Leiguarda and Marsden, 2000). Although these sensorimotor and cognitive disorders may coexist with limb apraxia, the clinician must first ascertain whether they account for the patient’s inability to perform learned skilled movements.


Limb apraxia is not rare or insignificant. Apraxia occurs in about 50% to 80% of patients with left hemisphere lesions and can persist as a chronic deficit in 40% to 50% of these. Limb apraxia often results in major functional impairment, even when subtle, as it affects critical movements of the arms, hands, and fingers. Limb apraxia correlates with the level of caregiver dependence and greater need for help with activities of daily living (ADLs). It can also interfere with rehabilitation and therapy, including occupational therapy, physical therapy, and the use of gestural communication in aphasia rehabilitation.


Despite its importance, clinicians often fail to recognize limb apraxia. In many left hemisphere strokes, right hemiparesis masks the presence of right limb apraxia, and the assumption of normal non-dominant hand clumsiness masks the presence of left limb apraxia. Even when there are no masking factors, the presence of limb apraxia may still go undetected. Many examiners do not evaluate patients for limb apraxia, do not know how to test for apraxia, or cannot recognize the spatiotemporal or content errors produced by this condition. Yet apraxia is part of the diagnostic criteria of Alzheimer disease and corticobasal syndrome, as well as a manifestation of many other disorders.


This chapter is about the limb apraxias. The term apraxia occurs broadly in neurology and is usually interchangeable with dyspraxia. Apraxia describes non-learned motor dysfunctions including oculomotor movements, gait initiation (magnetic apraxia), and eyelid opening. It also describes skilled motor tasks that are dependent on visuospatial processing, including optic, constructional, and dressing apraxia. Apraxia is used as well for conditions that are more clearly consistent with the definition of disturbances in learned skilled movements but involve body parts other than the limbs, including orobucchal-facial and speech apraxias. These clinical entities are not included in this chapter, because they are either not limb apraxias or not disorders of “praxis” in the sense of disturbances in learned skilled movements (Zadikoff and Lang, 2005). This chapter focuses on the seven major limb apraxias of the upper extremities, where apraxia is most evident. They include ideomotor apraxia, parietal variant; ideomotor apraxia, disconnection variant; dissociation apraxia; ideational apraxia; and conceptual apraxia. Also included is limb-kinetic apraxia, a disorder that some argue is not a true apraxia, but instead a more basic disturbance in fine motor movements. Callosal apraxias comprise a separate category because of their unique unilateral and varied manifestations.



Historical Perspective


Many clinicians and investigators helped develop the current concept of limb apraxia. In 1866, John Hughlings Jackson probably recognized the clinical phenomenon of apraxia in a patient (Pearce, 2009). Jackson observed that the patient had “power in his muscles and in the centres for coordination of muscular groups, but he – the whole man, or the ‘will’ – cannot set them agoing.” In 1870, Carl Maria Finkelnburg used “asymbolia” to describe the clumsy and incomprehensible communicative gestures in aphasics, and in 1890, Meynert distinguished motor asymbolia from decreased motor “images” for movement. In 1899, D. De Buck used “parakinesia” to describe a patient who “though retaining the concepts for her actions, did not succeed in awakening the corresponding kinetic image.” By this time, the stage was set for Hugo Karl Liepmann’s seminal model of the limb apraxias.


In the early 1900s, Liepmann published a series of papers that led to the contemporary concept of limb apraxias. He proposed that the execution of purposeful movements could be divided into three steps (Goldenberg, 2003). First is the retrieval of the spatial and temporal representation or “movement formulas” of the intended action from the left hemisphere. Second is the transfer and association of these movement formulas via cortical connections with the “innervatory patterns” or motor programs located in the left “sensomotorium” (which includes premotor and supplementary motor areas). Third is the transmission of the information to the left primary motor cortex for performance of the intended actions in the right limb. Finally, in order for the left limb to perform the movements, the information traverses the corpus callosum to the right sensomotorium to activate the right primary motor cortex. Using Heymann Steinthal’s term of “apraxia,” Liepmann classified disturbances in these connections as “ideational, ideo-kinetic (melokinetic), and limb-kinetic apraxia.” Over the years, this classification nomenclature has evolved and the application of these terms has shifted, but Liepmann’s basic formulation of apraxia has persisted to the present day.



A Model for Praxis


The left parietal region retains its central role of converting mental images of intended action into motor execution (Heilman and Rothi, 2003) (Fig. 10.1). The inferior parietal lobule contains the spatial and temporal movement programs (praxicons, visuokinesthetic motor engrams, or movement formulas) needed to carry out learned skilled movements. Multiple input modalities including visual, verbal-auditory, and tactile can activate these movement formulas. Cells in the inferior parietal lobule fire selectively in response to hand movements, visually presented information about object size and shape, or the actual manipulation of objects (Rizzolatti et al., 1998), and functional neuroimaging studies show activity of this region in response to recognition of actions associated with object or tool use (transitive actions) (Damasio et al., 2001). The right parietal lobe participates in the integration of visual information and upper-extremity movement. In addition to movement formulas, the left parietal region appears to contain action semantics and conceptual systems such as tool action, tool-object association information, and general principles of tool use (Goldenberg and Spatt, 2009; Ochipa et al., 1992). If a movement involves the use of a tool or object, action semantics specify knowledge of tool action (turning, pounding, etc.) and the knowledge of which tool or object to use to choose for a task (Leiguarda and Marsden, 2000).



In the premotor region, the supplementary motor area (SMA) translates the movement formulas into motor programs before sending them on to primary motor cortex (Roy and Square, 1985). The SMA, which is involved in complex movements of the upper extremities, receives projections from parietal neurons and in turn projects axons to motor neurons in the primary motor cortex. The SMA programs a specific order of movements and is involved in bimanual coordination. It translates these time-space movement formulas to specific motor programs that activate the motor neurons such that the contralateral extremity moves in the proscribed spatial trajectory and timing. For movements in the opposite extremity, the brain further conveys these programs across the corpus callosum to the opposite premotor cortex and activates the motor neurons for the desired contralateral extremity movements.


Beyond this traditional model for praxis, apraxia may result from damage in other regions including the prefrontal cortex, right hemisphere, basal ganglia (putamen and globus pallidus), thalamus, and their white-matter connections. The prefrontal region participates in sequencing multiple arm, hand, and finger movements. The right parietal region participates in performing nonpurposeful movements. Although the left inferior parietal lobule is more active than the right during action imagery and actual discrimination of nonpurposeful gestures, the right parietal region is more active during imitation and when these gestures consist of finger postures (Buccino et al., 2001; Hermsdorfer et al., 2001). The role of basal ganglia and thalamus is less clear, but they function as part of cortical-subcortical motor loops. Apraxia could, theoretically, result from damage to any of these areas outside the traditional model of praxis.



Classification of Limb Apraxias


Beginning with Liepmann, there have been multiple attempts to classify and define the limb apraxias (Hanna-Pladdy and Rothi, 2001). The classification presented here is based on the seminal work of Heilman and associates, who have significantly contributed to the understanding of the limb apraxias (Heilman and Rothi, 2003). Depending on the location of the lesion, the patient has different patterns of ability to imitate and recognize gestures, perform sequential movements, and do fine motor activities (Fig. 10.2). The presence of production and content errors further characterize the subtypes of limb apraxia.




Ideomotor Apraxia, Parietal Variant


The parietal variant of ideomotor apraxia may be the most common and prototypical limb apraxia. Disruption of the movement formulas in the inferior parietal lobule impairs skilled movements on command and to imitation, as well as the recognition of gestures (see Fig. 10.2, A). Patients make spatial and temporal errors while producing movements. There is a failure to adopt the correct posture or orientation of the arm and hand or to move the limb correctly in space and at the correct speeds. Spatial errors involve the configuration of the hand and fingers, the proper orientation of the limb to the tool or object, and the spatial trajectory of the motion. A major distinguishing feature of the parietal variant of ideomotor apraxia is difficulty recognizing or identifying gestures, implicating damage to the praxicons, visuokinesthetic motor engrams, or movement formulas themselves.



Ideomotor Apraxia, Disconnection Variant


The disconnection variant of ideomotor apraxia results from disruptions of motor programs in the SMA or in their intra- and interhemispheric connections (Heilman and Watson, 2008). This form of ideomotor apraxia is a disconnection of an intact parietal region from the pathways to primary motor cortices. These lesions result in impaired pantomime to verbal commands, impaired imitation of gestures, and the presence of spatiotemporal production errors. The movement formulas themselves are preserved, but in contrast to the parietal variant of ideomotor apraxia, these patients can recognize and identify gestures. The lesions lie along the route from the left inferior parietal cortex to primary motor cortices (see Fig. 10.2, B). Although SMA lesions tend to affect both upper extremities, if the SMA lesion is limited to the right, apraxia may be limited to the left upper extremity.


< div class='tao-gold-member'>

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Jun 19, 2016 | Posted by in NEUROLOGY | Comments Off on Limb Apraxias and Related Disorders

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access